Predicting High-risk Behaviors Based on Time Perspective in Iranian Adolescents by Emphasizing the Mediating Role of Decision Making and Inhibitory Response: Path Analysis

authors:

avatar Fatemeh Bagherian ORCID 1 , * , avatar Nazir Mozafari ORCID 2 , avatar Ali Zadeh Mohammadi ORCID 3 , avatar Mahmood Heidari ORCID 2

Faculty of Psychology and Education, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, IR Iran
Faculty of Psychology, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, IR Iran
Family Research Institute, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, IR Iran

how to cite: Bagherian F, Mozafari N, Zadeh Mohammadi A, Heidari M. Predicting High-risk Behaviors Based on Time Perspective in Iranian Adolescents by Emphasizing the Mediating Role of Decision Making and Inhibitory Response: Path Analysis. Int J High Risk Behav Addict. 2022;11(1):e114519. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijhrba.114519.

Abstract

Background:

Adolescence is defined as a period with multiple and intertwined physical, psychological, and social developments during which risky behaviors increases. Various factors affect the tendency and occurrence of risky behaviors. Time perspective (TP) is defined as the role of time and attitude toward time in affairs and behaviors. Executive functioning skills allow an individual to perceive stimuli from his or her environment, respond adaptively, flexibly change direction, anticipate future goals, consider consequences, and respond in an integrated or commonsense way.

Objectives:

This study aimed to predict risky behaviors based on TP in Iranian adolescents by emphasizing the mediating role of decision making and inhibitory response.

Materials and Methods:

Path analysis was used to evaluate the relationship between components of TP and risky behaviors with mediator variables, decision making, and inhibitory response among 804 high school adolescent students (including 470 males and 334 females) in Sanandaj, Iran. All participants were asked to complete the Iranian Adolescent Risk Scale and Time Perspective Scale and perform 2 computer tasks [i.e., Go/No-Go and Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART)]. Statistical methods were descriptive statistics, correlation test, as well as path analysis. Also, the bootstrapping procedure was used. The output provided a 95% CI of the indirect effects. SPSS version 26 and AMOS version 24 were used to analyze direct and indirect relationships.

Results:

There was a significant relationship between the components of TP and the tendency to risky behaviors. Risky decision-making and inhibitory response had a significant effect on 3 aspects of TP in predicting adolescents’ risky behaviors. These effects were both direct and indirect, of which the former effect was created by mediating the decision-making and inhibitory response.

Conclusions:

The components of TP, decision-making, and inhibitory response predict risky behaviors in adolescents.

1. Background

What threatens most adolescents today are social morbidities, including a range of high-risk, harmful behaviors (1). The self-centeredness of adolescence makes them feel unique and take dangerous actions based on this personal fable that they are invulnerable. The occurrence and persistence of these behaviors cause serious physical, psychological, and sociological consequences and reduce their quality of life now and in the future (2-5). This has caused serious concern for families and the community (4). By presenting the problem behavior syndrome, Jessor identified risky behaviors, including smoking, drug use, alcohol, dangerous driving, and early sexual behaviors (6). Although adults also experience high-risk behaviors, they are more common in adolescence, so that these behaviors increase from early adolescence to mid-adolescence and decrease in late adolescence (5, 7-9). Numerous studies have shown that the prevalence of risky behaviors among Iranian adolescents increases (10-12).

Zimbardo and Boyd empirically distinguished 5 time perspectives (TPs): (1) past-positive (nostalgic, sentimental view of the past); (2) past-negative (negative, pessimistic attitude toward the past); (3) present-fatalistic (helpless and hopeless attitude toward the future and life), (4) present-hedonistic (present pleasure, immediate gratification, impulsive, risk-taking); and (5) future (striving for future goals) (13-16). A large amount of empirical data confirms that TP dimensions predict numerous fundamental life outcomes, including risky behaviors such as smoking (17), alcohol consumption (18), substance use (19), sexual risky behaviors (20), unsafe and risky driving (21), and gambling (22).

Executive function (EF) has become an umbrella term used for a diversity of hypothesized cognitive processes, including planning, working memory, attention, inhibition, self-monitoring, self-regulation, set-shifting, decision-making, and initiation carried out by prefrontal areas of the frontal lobes (23-25). Some researchers refer to EFs as the conductor of an orchestra, which is responsible for directing and organizing all human actions (24). In this study, 2 kinds of EFs were discussed: decision-making and response inhibition.

Inhibition is often considered an executive functioning ability or process. Response inhibition is defined as the ability to reject an automatic tendency in a given situation (26). People with low-inhibition act impulsively are more likely to engage in risky behaviors, such as substance use (27-29), alcohol abuse (30, 31), sexual risk behaviors (32, 33), and risky driving (34, 35).

A decision is defined as a commitment to take action. In making a decision, a person in a situation chooses among the options available based on his/her available information (36). In this choice, items such as probabilities and costs/benefits are discussed (37). Some researchers consider decision-making to be one of the hot EFs. If hot EF involves decision-making in highly charged emotional contexts, typified by reinforcement and motivational forces, real-world risk-taking contexts should provide a fertile field to observe hot EF in action during adolescence (38). Studies have shown that risky decision-making is more likely to occur in adolescence than in other developmental periods, and risk-taking decreases with age (39).

EFs cover a wide range of cognitive abilities of the brain. Therefore, in this study, the mediating role of decision-making and inhibitory response regarding the relationship between TP and risky behaviors was examined. This study investigated the possibility of predicting risky behaviors based on TP components with an emphasis on the role of EF (decision-making and inhibitory response) as a mediator between Iranian adolescents.

The present study examined 2 hypotheses: (1) decision-making plays a mediating role between TP and risky behaviors; and (2) inhibitory response plays a mediating role between TP and risky behaviors.

2. Objectives

Considering the roles of TP and EF in risky behaviors and given that risky behaviors are one of the fundamental adolescent health issues, the present study aimed to examine the direct and indirect effects of components of TP on risky behaviors based on the mediating role of decision-making and inhibitory response, which has not received much attention in Iran.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Subjects

Male and female high school adolescents from public high schools in Sanandaj, Iran, were enrolled in this study between 2019 and 2020.

Sampling Method: In this study, a multi-stage cluster sampling method was used for hypothesis testing of a total of 804 students, including 470 (58.5%) males and 334 (41.5%) females. Subjects were selected based on the inclusion criteria, including age between 14 - 18 years, tendency to participate in the study, and the ability to speak and write Farsi sufficiently. Exclusion criteria were having a history of psychiatric (bipolar disorder or major depression and psychoses) or neurological disorders and using antidepressants and other psychiatric drugs during the study. Among the participants, 14 (9 boys and 5 girls) were excluded from the study due to deficiencies in completing the questionnaires.

3.2. Research Tools

In order to measure the variables, the following tools were used in this study.

3.2.1. Iranian Adolescent Risk Scale

This scale consists of 38 items, and its purpose is to measure the risk of adolescents in 7 dimensions: drug (8 items), alcohol (6 items), smoking (5 items), violence (5 items), sexual relationship and behavior (4 items), relationship with the opposite sex (4 items), and dangerous driving (6 items). Its scoring method is based on the Likert scale with 5 options from completely agree (5) to completely disagree (1); it measures the level of risk at 3 levels: (1) low, (2) medium, and (3) high (12). In this study, its reliability in assessing the Iranian Adolescent Risk Scale was calculated at 0.94 using the Cronbach α, which was significant (P = 0.005).

3.2.2. Time Perspective Scale

This scale was designed by Zimbardo to examine people’s tendency to past, present, and future; this scale consists of 56 items, including 5 subscales of future (13 items), past-positive (9 items), past-negative (10 items), present-hedonistic (15 items), and present-fatalistic (9 items) (16). Its scoring method is based on the Likert scale with 5 options from completely true (5) to completely false (1). The scoring method for items 9, 24, 25, 41, 56, and 61 is inverted. Respondents rate their degree of endorsement of each statement on a 5-point Likert-type response scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Zimbardo and Boyd reported the Cronbach α for subscales 0.80, 0.82, 0.79, 0.78, and 0.74 (15).

3.2.3. Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART)

In this task, which is used to measure decision-making, an image of a balloon appears on a computer screen that the subject can inflate the balloon by pressing a button below it. There are 2 temporary and permanent boxes on the screen. Each time the balloon is inflated, a point is credited to the ballot box. Instead of inflating the balloon more, one can press the scoring key. At this time, a new balloon is replaced, and the amount saved from inflating the balloon goes to the permanent box. The total number of balloons is 30. Balloons burst at an unspecified point, and this makes high-risk decisions possible (40). The reliability of this task was reported by White et al. as 0.77 after retesting (41).

3.2.4. Go/No-Go Task

In this task, which is used to assess inhibition, participants are instructed to respond by pressing a button with their right index finger as accurately and quickly as possible to Go stimuli (85% probability) and to withhold a response to No-Go stimuli (15% probability). Go and No-Go stimuli are presented for 45milliseconds. Results are presented only for No-Go correct rejections (successful inhibitions) and No-Go false alarms (unsuccessful inhibitions) (42). The reliability coefficients of this test were reported by Ghadiri et al. between 0.72 and 0.78 (43).

3.3. Data Analysis

Statistical methods for data analysis were descriptive statistics, including frequency, mean, SD, minimum, maximum, and correlations between variables, as well as path analysis for direct and indirect effects. All analyses were performed using SPSS version 26 and AMOS version 25 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill, USA).

To investigate whether the components of EF acted as mediators in the relationship between TP and risky behaviors, the bootstrapping procedure for mediators was used. AMOS was used to test the indirect effects of EF. The output provided a 95% CI of the indirect effects.

4. Results

Examination of the demographic characteristics of the sample showed that in terms of gender, 470 (58.5%) were boys, and 334 (41.5%) were girls and, in terms of age, 158 (19.7%), 372 (46.3%), and 274 (34.1%) were 15, 16, and 17 years old, respectively. Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of the research variables.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of Research Variables

VariablesMean ± SDMinMax
Risky behaviors90.72 ± 31.2044171
Drug 13.83 ± 5.58835
Alcohol14.18 ± 6.10630
Smoking11.03 ± 5.68525
Violence13.49 ± 4.53525
Sexual relationship and behavior8.62 ± 4.13418
Relationship with the opposite sex13.26 ± 4.42420
Dangerous driving16.27 ± 5.67730
Past-Positive 28 ± 41341
Past-Negative 27 ± 81350
Present-Hedonistic46 ± 131771
Present-Fatalistic24 ± 6.061340
Future 40.1 ± 10.12358
BART (response inhibition)8177 ± 1125101010100
Go/No-Go task (decision-making)67 ± 92579

Table 2 displays correlations between various variables included in this study. A number of conclusions can be drawn from this table. The findings indicated that there was a significant relationship between all variables with risky behaviors.

Table 2.

Matrix Correlations Between all Variables a

Variables12345678
1. Risky behaviors1
2. Past-Positive-0.484 **1
3. Past-Negative0.529 **-0.571 **1
4. Future-0.671 **0.738 **-0.539 **1
5. Present-Hedonistic0.746 **-0.565 **0.751 **-0.708 **1
6. Present-Fatalistic0.336 **-0.0250.310 **-0.197 **0.452 **1
7. BART-0.575 **0.170 **-0.284 **0.310 **-0.480 **-0.386 **1
8. Go/No-Go task-0.528 **0.280 **-0.356 **0.304 **-0.473 **-0.256 **0.495 **1

4.1. The Mediating Role of Decision-making

The results of the mediation analyses are presented in Table 3. The total effects of Future (β = -1.005; P = 0.000) and Present-Hedonistic (β = 1.338; P = 0.001) were significant, but the total effects of past-positive (β = 0.303; P = 0.32), Past-Negative (β = -0.230; P = 0.18), and present-fatalistic (β = 0.169; P = 0.37) were not significant. The direct effects of future (β = -1.09; P = 0.001) and present-hedonistic (β = 1.064; P = 0.001) were significant, but the direct effects of past-positive (β = 0.469; P = 0.171), past-negative (β = -0.205; P = 0.351), and present-fatalistic (β = 0.090; P = 0.52) were not significant (Figure 1).

Table 3.

Results of the Bootstrapping Analyses Testing Decision-making as Mediators Between TP Components and Risky Behaviors

VariablesTotal EffectIndirect EffectDirect Effect
Past-positive → risky behavior0.303 (0.322)-0.167 (0.037)0.469 (0.171)Indirect effect
Past-negative → risky behavior-0.230 (0.180)-0.025 (0.647)-0.205 (0.351)NS
Future → risky behavior-1.005 (0.001)0.106 (0.031)-1.091 (0.001)Partial mediation
Present-hedonistic → risky behavior1.338 (0.001)0.247 (0.001)1.064 (0.001)Partial mediation
Present-fatalistic → risky behavior0.169 (0.378)0.079 (0.258)0.090 (0.520)NS
Decision-making as a mediator between past-positive, past-negative, future, present-hedonistic, and present-fatalistic TP and risky behaviors
Decision-making as a mediator between past-positive, past-negative, future, present-hedonistic, and present-fatalistic TP and risky behaviors

4.2. The Mediating Role of Response Inhibition

The results of the mediation analyses are presented in Table 4. The total effects of future (β = -1.005; P = 0.000) and present-hedonistic (β = 1.338; P = 0.001) were significant, but the total effects of past-positive (β = 0.303; P = 0.32), past-negative (β = -0.230; P = 0.18), and present-fatalistic (β = 0.169; P = 0.37) were not significant. The direct effects of future (β = -0.97; P = 0.001) and present-hedonistic (β = 0.986; P = 0.001) were significant, but the direct effects of past-positive (β = 0.159; P = 0.736), past-negative (β = -0.076; P = 0.773), and present-fatalistic (β = -0.121; P = 0.578) were not significant (Figure 2).

Table 4.

Results of the Bootstrapping Analyses Testing Inhibition as Mediators Between TP Components and Risky Behaviors

VariablesTotal EffectIndirect EffectDirect Effect
Past-positive → risky behavior0.303 (0.322)0.143 (0.267)0.159 (0.736)NS
Past-negative → risky behavior-0.230 (0.180)-0.155 (0.115)-0.076 (0.773)NS
Future → risky behavior-1.005 (0.001)-0.036 (0.554)-0.97 (0.001)Direct effect
Present-hedonistic → risky behavior1.338 (0.001)0.325 (0.001)0.986 (0.001)Partial mediation
Present-fatalistic → risky behavior0.169 (0.378)0.290 (0.001)-0.121 (0.578)Indirect effect
Response inhibition as a mediator between past-positive, past-negative, future, present-hedonistic, and present-fatalistic TP components and risky behaviors
Response inhibition as a mediator between past-positive, past-negative, future, present-hedonistic, and present-fatalistic TP components and risky behaviors

5. Discussion

According to the results, there was a considerable association between decision-making, response inhibition, TP, and risky behaviors. Results also showed that TP could predict risky behaviors through the mediation role of decision-making and response inhibition. Previous studies have shown the relationship between TP and risky behaviors. Our findings are in line with some of the previous studies. For instance, Konowalczyk et al. showed that adolescents who had a positive perspective exercised more and had more self-esteem and, as a result, did not seek to engage in risky behaviors (44). Xu et al. found in a cross-sectional study of adolescents that future perspective was less likely to lead to smoking and, in fact, was a protective component (17). Paasche et al. showed that hedonistic and futuristic perspectives had significant positive and negative relationships with substance use (19).

Also, Lemarie et al. showed that hedonistic perspective increased the likelihood of high-risk driving, and these people experienced things like speeding and overtaking while driving (21). On the other hand, TP has a significant relationship with psychological variables such as self-confidence and hope that can affect inhibition and decision-making (45). TP is also associated with engagement and commitment to homework, to the point where futurists are more motivated to engage in healthy activities and focus on long-term benefits such as health and success, and destiny-oriented people with present perspective believe in the role of fate and destiny, others, and luck (14).

In this regard, previous studies have shown the relationship between EF and risky behaviors. Our findings are in line with some of the previous studies. For instance, Leshem and King showed that risky behaviors were associated with the Go/No‐Go and BART (46). In addition, Pentz et al. found in a cross-sectional study of adolescent smokers that the deficit of EF was associated with an increased likelihood of smoking (47). Also, the results of Hayashi et al. showed that students with a low level of EF were more likely to engage in dangerous driving behaviors and experience negative driving outcomes (48).

According to Miyake and Friedman, inhibition is a key component of other EFs, demonstrating the ability to voluntarily control automatic and dominant responses in emergencies and can keep the individual alert to the possibility of danger and facilitate avoidant behaviors (49). Poor inhibition is significantly associated with impulsivity and causes adolescents to engage in high-risk behaviors without considering all precautions. Decision-making, on the other hand, is the process by which the option that has the highest and fastest returns is selected. This seemingly simple process has complexities, such as the conflict between values, choices, and the role of emotions in the final choice (38). In high-risk decision-making, a behavior that has immediate pleasure and benefit is preferred to other actions and behaviors. Although logic is the basis of EFs, the role of emotions is more prominent in high-risk decisions (50).

Although the TP is defined as a cognitive process in the formation and regulation of experiences over time, there are few empirical studies on its relationship with basic cognitive processes (50). However, some dimensions of TP may potentially be related to EF. In explaining the mediating role of decision-making and inhibition regarding TP and risky behaviors, it can be said that TP is considered as a process related to emotional, motivational, and functional factors; however, it is considered more as a cognitive schema that indicates its possible relationships with other cognitive functions (14, 15). One of the basic mechanisms in decision-making is time perception.

In high-risk decision-making, adolescents believe that they can enjoy the present by engaging in enjoyable, albeit risky activities, and consequently, they ignore the consequences of such behaviors because they are not available (51). Studies have shown that foresight may be an important component of self-regulation in some areas, such as achieving health-related goals and behaviors (52). In fact, foresight is associated with delays in immediate satisfaction of needs, low impulsivity, and rational decision-making, which can be considered as examples of self-regulation (53). On the other hand, high-risk behaviors have a certain excitement. Due to the role of emotions in high-risk decisions, risk-taking adolescents tend to engage in risky behaviors. Foresight and hedonism can also be expected to be related to decision-making and inhibition and, it causes adolescents' decisions emotional-based and to involve them in risky behaviors (32).

Present fatalism is defined as a frustrating attitude with a low level of control over events by the person and a belief in the power of the role of destiny in life that can influence a person’s decisions so that the person takes himself less responsible for his own behavior, and such a view can lead to a tendency for adolescents to engage in risky behaviors (15). Negative TP focuses on the role of unpleasant past events and negative interpretations of past events that are associated with negative emotions, psychotic symptoms, depression, and anxiety. These negative emotions are associated with poor inhibition and can affect the performance of risky behaviors (54).

Finally, it should be said that both futurism and inhibition have common principles of neuropsychology so that the role of the prefrontal is dominant in both, which is effective in deciding to perform healthy or high-risk behaviors. The prefrontal cortex is the last brain area to fully mature. By showing the mediating role of decision-making and inhibition in risky behaviors, this study demonstrated the predictive role of TP. Thus, TP and cognitive rehabilitation interventions could be considered preventive programs to help risky adolescents and reduce the possibility of risky behaviors in this sensitive group.

Considering the mediating role of EF in the relationship between TP and risky behaviors, researchers are advised to examine the effectiveness of TP interventions or cognitive rehabilitation in reducing adolescent high-risk behaviors.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    DiClemente RJ, Hansen WB, Ponton LE. Handbook of adolescent health risk behavior. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer Science & Business Media; 2013.

  • 2.

    Shaffer DR, Kipp K. Developmental psychology: Childhood and adolescence. Massachusetts, USA: Cengage Learning; 2013.

  • 3.

    Kazemzadeh Y, Shokoohi M, Baneshi MR, Haghdoost AA. The Frequency of High-Risk Behaviors Among Iranian College Students Using Indirect Methods: Network Scale-Up and Crosswise Model. Int J High Risk Behav Addict. 2016;5(3). e25130. [PubMed ID: 27818962]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5086407]. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijhrba.25130.

  • 4.

    Samadypoor R, Kord Tamini B. The Role of Personality Pattern Behaviors in Risk Behaviors of High School Students. International Journal of High Risk Behaviors and Addiction. 2016;5(4). e36313. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijhrba.36313.

  • 5.

    Fallahtafti S, Ghanbaripirkashani N, Alizadeh SS, Rovoshi RS. Psychometric Properties of the Smartphone Addiction Scale – Short Version (SAS-SV) in a Sample of Iranian Adolescents. Int J Dev Sci. 2020;14(1-2):19-26. https://doi.org/10.3233/dev-200002.

  • 6.

    Jessor R. Risk behavior in adolescence: a psychosocial framework for understanding and action. J Adolesc Health. 1991;12(8):597-605. [PubMed ID: 1799569]. https://doi.org/10.1016/1054-139x(91)90007-k.

  • 7.

    Abbasi-Ghahramanloo A, Heshmat R, Safiri S, Esmaeil Motlagh M, Ardalan G, Mahdavi-Gorabi A, et al. Risk-Taking Behaviors in Iranian Children and Adolescents: A Latent Class Analysis Approach: Caspian IV Study. J Res Health Sci. 2018;18(4). e00428. [PubMed ID: 30728314]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6941635].

  • 8.

    Peeters M, Oldehinkel A, Veenstra R, Vollebergh W. Unique developmental trajectories of risk behaviors in adolescence and associated outcomes in young adulthood. PLoS One. 2019;14(11). e0225088. [PubMed ID: 31721787]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6853606]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225088.

  • 9.

    Rehm J, Guiraud J, Poulnais R, Shield KD. Alcohol dependence and very high risk level of alcohol consumption: A life-threatening and debilitating disease. Addict Biol. 2018;23(4):961-8. [PubMed ID: 30043407]. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12646.

  • 10.

    Mirzaei Poueenak F, Ghanbari Pirkashani N, Nooripour R, Hosseini SR, Mazloomzadeh M, Shirkhani M. Psychometric validation of the Persian version of the community assessment of psychotic experiences-42 (CAPE-42) in Iranian college students. Psychosis. 2021:1-12. https://doi.org/10.1080/17522439.2020.1861075.

  • 11.

    Khodadadi Sangdeh J, Ahmadi K, Malazmani A, Albokordi S. Prevalence of high-risk behaviors in adolescent girls and boys of military families. Mil Psychol. 2013;9:19-28. https://doi.org/10.26719/2007.13.5.1017.

  • 12.

    Zadeh Mohammadi A, Ahmadabadi Z, Heidari M. [Development and evaluation of psychometric properties of Iranian adolescents' risk scale]. Thought and Behavior. 2012;3:218-25. Persian.

  • 13.

    Mischel W. The marshmallow test: Why self-control is the engine of success. New York, USA: Little, Brown; 2015.

  • 14.

    Nooripour R, Ghanbari N, Hoseinian S, Vakili Y, Dobkins K. Psychometric Validation of the Farsi Version of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) in a Sample of Iranian Students in the USA. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-021-00617-9.

  • 15.

    Habibi M, Alahdadi S, Mohammadi L, Ghanbari N. Psychometric Properties of Behavioral Activation/Inhibition Systems (BAS/BIS) in Dependent People with Drug and Alcohol. Health. 2019;10(1):58-72. https://doi.org/10.29252/j.health.10.1.58.

  • 16.

    Zimbardo P, Boyd J. The time paradox: The new psychology of time that will change your life. New York, USA: Simon and Schuster; 2008.

  • 17.

    Xu Y, Yang Y, Ma X. Time perspective, optimistic bias, and self-control among different statuses of university smokers in China: a cross-sectional study. Psychol Health Med. 2018;23(9):1054-9. [PubMed ID: 29313374]. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2018.1425461.

  • 18.

    Wagner V, Acier D, Dietlin JE. Mediation of time perspectives on inclinations to use alcohol and motivation to change relationship. J Clin Psychol. 2018;74(10):1854-66. [PubMed ID: 29756291]. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22637.

  • 19.

    Paasche C, Weibel S, Wittmann M, Lalanne L. Time perception and impulsivity: A proposed relationship in addictive disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;106:182-201. [PubMed ID: 30529361]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.006.

  • 20.

    Abousselam N, Naude L, Lens W, Esterhuyse K. The relationship between future time perspective, self-efficacy and risky sexual behaviour in the Black youth of central South Africa. J Ment Health. 2016;25(2):176-83. [PubMed ID: 26471935]. https://doi.org/10.3109/09638237.2015.1078884.

  • 21.

    Lemarie L, Bellavance F, Chebat JC. Regulatory focus, time perspective, locus of control and sensation seeking as predictors of risky driving behaviors. Accid Anal Prev. 2019;127:19-27. [PubMed ID: 30826693]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2019.02.025.

  • 22.

    Donati MA, Sottili E, Morsanyi K, Primi C. Time Perspectives and Gambling in Adolescent Boys: Differential Effects of Present- and Future-Orientation. J Gambl Stud. 2019;35(1):107-24. [PubMed ID: 29868999]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-018-9780-0.

  • 23.

    Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(3):174-80. [PubMed ID: 22336729]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006.

  • 24.

    McCloskey G, Perkins LA, Van Divner B. Assessment and intervention for executive function difficulties. Oxfordshire, UK: Taylor & Francis; 2008. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203893753.

  • 25.

    Mace RA, Waters AB, Sawyer KS, Turrisi T, Gansler DA. Components of executive function model regional prefrontal volumes. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(7):1007-19. [PubMed ID: 31512888]. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000563.

  • 26.

    Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-68. [PubMed ID: 23020641]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4084861]. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

  • 27.

    Byrne KA, Worthy DA. Examining the link between reward and response inhibition in individuals with substance abuse tendencies. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2019;194:518-25. [PubMed ID: 30544087]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6340392]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.11.014.

  • 28.

    Piche J, Kaylegian J, Smith D, Hunter SJ. The Relationship between Self-Reported Executive Functioning and Risk-Taking Behavior in Urban Homeless Youth. Behav Sci (Basel). 2018;8(1). [PubMed ID: 29301347]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5791024]. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8010006.

  • 29.

    Reynolds BW, Basso MR, Miller AK, Whiteside DM, Combs D. Executive function, impulsivity, and risky behaviors in young adults. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(2):212-21. [PubMed ID: 30589284]. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000510.

  • 30.

    Keough MT, O'Connor RM. Clarifying the measurement and the role of the behavioral inhibition system in alcohol misuse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38(5):1470-9. [PubMed ID: 24588401]. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.12387.

  • 31.

    Camchong J, Endres M, Fein G. Decision making, risky behavior, and alcoholism. Handb Clin Neurol. 2014;125:227-36. [PubMed ID: 25307578]. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-62619-6.00014-8.

  • 32.

    Hansen NS, Thayer RE, Feldstein Ewing SW, Sabbineni A, Bryan AD. Neural Correlates of Risky Sex and Response Inhibition in High-Risk Adolescents. J Res Adolesc. 2018;28(1):56-69. [PubMed ID: 29460357]. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12344.

  • 33.

    Golub SA, Starks TJ, Kowalczyk WJ, Thompson LI, Parsons JT. Profiles of executive functioning: associations with substance dependence and risky sexual behavior. Psychol Addict Behav. 2012;26(4):895-905. [PubMed ID: 22775771]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3540196]. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029034.

  • 34.

    Sani SRH, Tabibi Z, Fadardi JS, Stavrinos D. Aggression, emotional self-regulation, attentional bias, and cognitive inhibition predict risky driving behavior. Accid Anal Prev. 2017;109:78-88. [PubMed ID: 29049929]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2017.10.006.

  • 35.

    Walshe EA, Ward McIntosh C, Romer D, Winston FK. Executive Function Capacities, Negative Driving Behavior and Crashes in Young Drivers. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(11). [PubMed ID: 29143762]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5707953]. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111314.

  • 36.

    Yates JF, de Oliveira S. Culture and decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2016;136:106-18. [PubMed ID: 32288179]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7126161]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.05.003.

  • 37.

    Newell BR, Lagnado DA, Shanks DR. Straight choices: The psychology of decision making. 2nd ed. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press; 2015. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315727080.

  • 38.

    Nooripour R, Ghanbari N, Hossenzadeh S. The Effectiveness of Mindfulness-Based Training on Self-Harmful Behaviors and Protective Factors of Substance Abuse in adolescent at juvenile correction and rehabilitation center. Sci Qua Re on Addi. 2020;13(54):205-28.

  • 39.

    Van Leijenhorst L, Gunther Moor B, Op de Macks ZA, Rombouts SA, Westenberg PM, Crone EA. Adolescent risky decision-making: neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. Neuroimage. 2010;51(1):345-55. [PubMed ID: 20188198]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.038.

  • 40.

    Nejati V. Relationship between executive functions of the brain with high risk decision making in students. Behav Sci. 2013;11(4):170-278.

  • 41.

    White TL, Lejuez CW, de Wit H. Test-retest characteristics of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2008;16(6):565-70. [PubMed ID: 19086777]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4244869]. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014083.

  • 42.

    Langenecker SA, Zubieta JK, Young EA, Akil H, Nielson KA. A task to manipulate attentional load, set-shifting, and inhibitory control: Convergent validity and test-retest reliability of the Parametric Go/No-Go Test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29(8):842-53. [PubMed ID: 17852593]. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390601147611.

  • 43.

    Ghadiri F, Jazayeri A, Nomadic H, Judge Tabatabai M. Executive dysfunctions in schizophrenic patients. Cognitive Science News. 2007;8(3):11-24.

  • 44.

    Konowalczyk S, Rade FCA, Mello ZR. Time perspective, sports club membership, and physical self-concept among adolescents: A person-centered approach. J Adolesc. 2019;72:141-51. [PubMed ID: 30903931]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.02.008.

  • 45.

    Worrell FC, Mello ZR. Convergent and Discriminant Validity of Time Attitude Scores on the Adolescent Time Perspective Inventory. Diskurs Kindheits-und Jugendforschung/Discourse. 2009;4(2):185-95.

  • 46.

    Leshem R, King R. Trait impulsivity and callous-unemotional traits as predictors of inhibitory control and risky choices among high-risk adolescents. Int J Psychol. 2021;56(2):314-21. [PubMed ID: 32748423]. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12710.

  • 47.

    Pentz MA, Shin H, Riggs N, Unger JB, Collison KL, Chou CP. Parent, peer, and executive function relationships to early adolescent e-cigarette use: a substance use pathway? Addict Behav. 2015;42:73-8. [PubMed ID: 25462657]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4292878]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.10.040.

  • 48.

    Hayashi Y, Foreman AM, Friedel JE, Wirth O. Executive function and dangerous driving behaviors in young drivers. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2018;52:51-61. [PubMed ID: 31024220]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6477690]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2017.11.007.

  • 49.

    Miyake A, Friedman NP. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):8-14. [PubMed ID: 22773897]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3388901]. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458.

  • 50.

    George JM, Dane E. Affect, emotion, and decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2016;136:47-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.06.004.

  • 51.

    Ashare RL, Kable JW. Sex differences in time perception during smoking abstinence. Nicotine Tob Res. 2015;17(4):449-54. [PubMed ID: 25762755]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4432399]. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntu260.

  • 52.

    Habibi M, Alahdadi S, Mohammadi L, Ghanbari N. [Psychometric properties of Leeds Dependence Questionnaire (LDQ) in dependent people with drug and alcohol]. Pajoohandeh. 2016;21(3):153-60. Persian.

  • 53.

    Eysenck MW, Derakshan N. New perspectives in attentional control theory. Pers Individ Differ. 2011;50(7):955-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019.

  • 54.

    Tseferidi SI, Griva F, Anagnostopoulos F. Time to get happy: associations of time perspective with indicators of well-being. Psychol Health Med. 2017;22(5):618-24. [PubMed ID: 27560279]. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2016.1226508.