Comparison of Working Memory and Executive Function in Patients with Internet Addiction Disorder, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, and Normal Individuals

authors:

avatar Ali Reza Shafiee-Kandjani ORCID 1 , 2 , avatar Zahra Mohammadzadeh 3 , avatar Shahrokh Amiri ORCID 3 , * , avatar Asghar Arfaie 2 , avatar Parvin Sarbakhsh ORCID 4 , avatar Salman Safikhanlou 3

Road Traffic Injury Research Center, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, IR Iran
Faculty of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, IR Iran
Research Center of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, IR Iran
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Health. Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, IR Iran

how to cite: Shafiee-Kandjani A R, Mohammadzadeh Z, Amiri S, Arfaie A, Sarbakhsh P, et al. Comparison of Working Memory and Executive Function in Patients with Internet Addiction Disorder, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, and Normal Individuals. Int J High Risk Behav Addict. 2020;9(2):e98997. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijhrba.98997.

Abstract

Background:

Cognitive functions are higher-order cortical activities that include various parameters such as concentration, executive function, and working memory.

Objectives:

The purpose of this study was to compare cognitive functions in three groups of Adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Internet Addiction Disorder (IAD), and normal people.

Patients and Methods:

This descriptive-analytical study included patients with ADHD, IAD, and normal individuals (26 normal individuals and 27 patients in each of the IAD and ADHD groups) who were evaluated by Conners’ Adult Scale, General Health Questionnaire (GHQ), Young Internet Addiction Test, and Persian Paper and Pencil Cognitive Assessment Package (PCAP). Data were analyzed by SPSS 23 software. The ANOVA test with Bonferroni’s pairwise comparison and chi-square test were used for data analysis at a significance level of 0.05.

Results:

The results showed that the ADHD subjects scored higher (P < 0.01) on the GHQ test and its subscales, as well as the Conners’ test. Also, patients with ADHD had lower scores on attention, concentration, and working memory scales than IAD and normal counterparts (P < 0.01). Working memory was weaker in IAD patients than normal subjects and ADHD and cognitive function is more damaged than IAD.

Conclusions:

The results showed that patients with ADHD had higher levels of impairment in working memory than IAD patients. Meanwhile, cognitive deficits were more sensible in ADHD.

1. Background

Cognitive functions are higher-order functions in the brain that include various parameters such as concentration, executive function, and working memory (1). The neurocognitive function may be impaired in addiction (2). People with internet addiction disorder may suffer from other behavioral addictions such as gambling, sexual behavior, and compulsive shopping (3).

Internet Addiction Disorder (IAD) is defined as a person’s inability to control internet use, with parallel psycho-social dysfunctions, as well as problems in the workplace, school, and daily life (4). Internet addiction disorder has quickly become a common mental health issue in many countries and has, therefore, attracted a lot of attention (5). It is most prevalent in the late second and early third decades of life (6). The prevalence of IAD is from 2.4% (7) to 36.5% (8) among Asian adolescents and youths and 4.6% (9) up to 13.4% (10) in the same age groups in Europe. The standardized prevalence among Iranian populations varies between 3.6% (11) and 45.8% (12).

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neuro-developmental disorder with a pattern of impaired attention and increased impulsivity and hyperactivity seen from childhood to adulthood (13). In a study, the prevalence of ADHD was reported to be 3.8% (5.5% for men and 2% for women) in Iran (14). Hyperactivity is known to decrease with age, but irritability and emotional dysregulation may be the most prevalent manifestations in older patients. These symptoms can co-occur with behavioral addiction; for this reason, IAD is one of the most common comorbidities among patients with ADHD who show a higher intensity of internet addiction (15). Moreover, IAD is found in both hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive types, although the former one is at a higher risk of addiction (16).

Zhou et al. concluded that IAD patients have differences in working memory and executive function, and impulsivity is higher in IAD patients (17). Park et al. observed that the earlier onset of IAD and its longer duration were associated with the lower performance of participants in attention tests (5).

2. Objectives

Several studies have pointed to a considerable cognitive deficit among people with Internet Use Disorder and ADHD compared to healthy individuals (18-20). There are yet insufficient data concerning differences between internet addiction and ADHD. In this spirit, the study aimed to determine the possible deficits in afflicted patients in an Iranian population.

3. Patients and Methods

This was a descriptive-analytical study with 30 people in each group. People with ADHD were conveniently selected from among 18 to 50-year-old patients who attended the psychiatric clinics of Tabriz University of Medical Sciences in Iran during 2017 - 2018 and met the inclusion criteria of the study. The IAD participants were conveniently recruited from among people attending internet and game cafes, or those who simply volunteered as patients for research upon an announcement; their age, sex, and education were matched to the ADHD group. The healthy participants were selected from among patients’ companions, hospital colleagues, or based on an announcement whose age, sex, and education were matched to the members of ADHD and IAD groups.

3.1. Data Collection Instruments

3.1.1. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID-IV)

This tool is a guide to make psychiatric diagnoses based on the diagnostic and statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). Sharifi et al. (21) confirmed its validity and reliability. Clinical interviews were done by an attending psychiatrist and SCID-IV-TR was performed for each participant by a single psychiatry resident.

3.1.2. Self-report Screening Version of the Conner’s Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS-S:SV)

It is a tool for screening adults with ADHD that uses four subscales. An Iranian study supported the high validity and reliability of the scale (22).

3.1.3. General Health Questionnaire (GHQ)

This questionnaire was developed by Goldberg and Hilder (1979, 23). It comprises the scales of physical symptoms, anxiety symptoms, sleep and social functioning disturbances, and depressive symptoms. Its validity and reliability have been confirmed by previous studies (23, 24).

3.1.4. Persian Paper and Pencil Cognitive Assessment Package (PCAP)

It is a cognitive test battery to assess working memory, attention, and executive functions. It includes Symbol/Letter Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Letter Number Sequencing Task (LNST), Trail Making Task, Verbal Fluency Task, and Stroop task. Rezapour et al. (25, 26) confirmed the validity and reliability of PCAP in an Iranian population.

3.1.5. Young Internet Addiction Test

The test was designed by Kimberly Young based on the DSM-IV-TR criteria (4, 27). Its Persian validation was confirmed by Mohammadsalehi et al. (28).

3.2. Inclusion Criteria

They included a diagnosis of ADHD and IAD according to the DSM-IV-TR criteria done by a psychiatrist and the age of 18 to 50 years.

3.3. Exclusion Criteria

They included brain or neurological disorders, comorbidity with major psychiatric illnesses, history of epilepsy, serious medical diseases, intellectual disability, taking drugs during the last 72 hours, and unwillingness to continue participating in the study.

The Forward Digit Span Test (FDST), Backward Digit Span Test (BDST), and Letter-Number Sequence Test (LNST) were used to evaluate working memory. Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Letter Digit Modalities Test (LDMT), and Verbal Fluency Task (VFT) were applied for executive function. The Trail Making Tests A and B (TMT A/TMT B) and Stroop Test (ST) were used to evaluate attention.

The chi-square test and one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) test were used to compare the variables between the three groups. A Bonferroni test was used for pairwise comparisons. The statistical analyses were performed at a significance level of 0.05 using SPSS 23 software.

The study protocol was approved by a regional ethics committee affiliated to Tabriz University of Medical Sciences under No. 94/3-9/15. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

4. Results

There were 26 normal individuals and 27 patients in each of the IAD and ADHD groups. The dropout rates were 10% for each of the ADHD and IAD groups and 13% for the healthy group.

4.1. Demographic Data

The average age was 29.9 ± 8.18 for all participants, 27.70 ± 7.22 in the IAD group, 31.44 ± 10.27 in the ADHD group, and 30.62 ± 6.27 for normal counterparts, with no statistically significant differences (Table 1).

Table 1.

Demographic Data

VariablesTotalNormal GroupADHD GroupInternet Add GroupChi-Square
FrequencyPercentage (%)FrequencyPercentage (%)FrequencyPercentage (%)FrequencyPercentage (%)
Gender0.04
Male 4961.251428.571938.781632.65
Female 3138.751238.71825.811135.48
Marital status0.37
Single44551329.551227.271943.18
Married 36451336.111541.67822.22
Education0.000
Elementary 11.2500110000
Middle school4500410000
High school1417.5428.57535.71535.71
Diploma 78.75114.29228.57457.14
Associate’s degree810337.5112.50450
Bachelor’s degree2632.5726.92934.621038.46
Master’s degree1316.25753.85323.08323.08
Doctorate 78.75457.14228.57114.29
Job-status0.000
Unemployed 1417.5214.29750535.71
Housewife911.25222.22444.44333.33
Employed 24301250312.50937.50
Self-employed2129.25628.571047.62523.81
Student 1215433.33325541.67
Family history0.000
No family history6378.752438.101726.982234.92
Psychiatric history1518.75213.33853.33533.33
Genetic history11.2500110000
Unknown11.2500110000
History of another medical condition0.000
No history7188.752230.992332.392636.62
Cancer 11.2500110000
GI11.2500110000
Renal22.500210000
Endocrine 11.2500110000
Neurological 4537500125
Substance use0.000
No history6277.52337.101829.032133.87
Nicotine and tobacco1620318.75743.75637.50
Opium11.2500110000
Stimulant11.2500110000

The analysis of variance showed significant differences between the three groups on GHQ, Young, and Conner’s tests (Table 2). Patients with ADHD scored higher on GHQ and its subscales, as well as the Conner’s scale, revealing that they had significantly more severe problems than the other two groups. In terms of sleep problems (P = 0.03), social function (P = 0.001), and the total score (P = 0.03), a significant difference was observed between the ADHD group and the other two groups. The IAD group had higher scores than the other two groups on the internet addiction test.

Table 2.

Descriptive Data of General Health, Young, and Conner’s Questionnaires and Analysis of Variance

QuestionnairesNormal GroupInternet Addiction GroupHyperactivity GroupFP Value
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
GHQ
Physical symptoms5.192.884.895.266.3784.391.490.23
Anxiety and sleep problems544.115.247.444.963.520.03
Social function7.383.415.075.309.703.168.71< 0.001
Depression symptoms2.193.1034.914.154.561.400.25
Total scores19.7710.7917.0718.8228.0714.573.870.03
Young Internet Addiction Test22.1516.9672.786.8928.8120.2581.67< 0.001
Conner’s Test22.5813.1324.8123.7644.0416.8110.89< 0.001

The analysis of variance using the Bonferroni post hoc test showed a significant difference in working memory (LNST) between the IAD group and healthy counterparts (P = 0.02). The ADHD group exhibited a significant difference from the normal group in all fields of working memory (the Longest Digit Span Forward and the Longest Digit Span Backward Tasks), executive function (VFT), and attention (ST third time error and interference score).

Significant differences were observed between the IAD and ADHD groups in all three fields of executive function (VFT), working memory (BDST), and attention (TMT B and Stroop test second time error, ST third time error, and ST interference time), with the sensible disorder being related to the ADHD group (Table 3).

Table 3.

Descriptive Data and Analysis of Variance of Persian Paper and Pencil Cognitive Assessment Package Questionnairesa

QuestionnairesNormal GroupInternet Addiction GroupHyperactivity GroupFP Value
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Working memory
Forward digit span task (FDST)9.62A2.128.44A1.728.37A2.23.140.05
Forward digit span task (FDST) longest chain6.27A1.285.63A1.045.59A1.282.620.08
Backward digit span task (BDST)7.96 A2.27.07 A1.995.44 B1.3412.3< 0.001
Backward digit span task (BDST), longest chain5.46AB1.484.89A1.154.22AC0.937.03< 0.001
Letter-number sequencing task (LNST)9.96 A2.848 B2.247.41 B2.457.44< 0.001
Symbol digit modality task (SDMT), (total)52.42 A11.4151.22 A14.8947.11 A11.921.250.29
symbol digit modality task (SDMT), (true reponses)52.04 A11.4950.78 A14.7146.41 A11.921.420.25
Letter digit modality task (LDMT), (total)52.92 A10.3455.11 A16.0949.15 A10.91.510.23
Letter digit modality task (LDMT), (true responses)52.92 A10.3455.11 A16.0948.85 A10.721.690.19
Executive function
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (P-letter)15.58 A4.417.41 A5.8311.33 B4.3810.8< 0.001
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (M-letter)16.04 A4.7118.19A5.4912.63B3.929.38< 0.001
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (K-letter)15.38 A4.0517.48A5.0111.81B4.610.57< 0.001
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (Animal)22 A4.7122.74 A5.1218.3 B5.395.9< 0.001
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (supermarket)20 A5.7320.93 A6.2817.3 A5.262.880.06
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (Fruit)17.62AB4.2816.63 A4.5314.81AC2.993.380.04
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (Letters)47 A11.7353.07A15.2735.78B12.0212.06< 0.001
Verbal fluency task (VFT), (Categories)59.62 A13.0660.3 A13.2450.41B10.765.330.01
Attention
Stroop task (ST), (first time)125 A28.85138.26A31.92126.59A32.981.410.25
Stroop task (ST), (first time, number of errors)0.38 A0.750.7 A1.680.33 A1.040.720.49
Stroop task (ST), (second time)116.38A45.03140.19AB37.3198.78AC37.957.22< 0.001
Stroop task (ST), (second time, number of errors)0.27 A0.720.78 A1.781.3 A3.861.110.33
Stroop task (ST), (third time)190.42 A39.52192.67 A34.14207.19 A71.880.840.44
Stroop task (ST), (third time, number of errors)0.73 A0.831.3 A2.114.89 B6.588.33< 0.001
Stroop task (ST), (interference time)3.81 A42.52-1.9332 b27.81 C25.925.78< 0.001
Trail making task (TMT), A27.04 A15.3328.15 A11.9429.04 A12.90.150.86
Trail making task (TMT), B58.27 A26.7551.44AB29.1875.15AC25.915.380.01

5. Discussion

In the analysis by Bonferroni test, the differences between the normal and IAD groups in the cognitive domains were not significant, except for LNST, which was related to working memory. In Park et al. (5) study, no difference was observed between IAD and normal participants in terms of working memory using the digit span task. In our study, working memory impairments were manifested in LNST. The DST consists of two forward and backward sub-tests, with no procedure performed on numbers in the first sub-test; however, the backward type provides a more accurate evaluation of cognitive status in question. In the Wechsler test, both LNST and BDST tests are used to evaluate working memory.

Wisdom et al. (29) concluded that LNST could show the least extent of cognitive changes, while the BDST showed small changes in the cognitive field. Similarly, in the study by Shelton et al. (30), it was observed that laboratory working memory tests such as the n-back test with LNST provided the best evaluation. The results of our study also pointed to the difference between LNST and BDST in working memory performance. Thus, it can be stated that LNST is more capable of revealing working memory problems than DST. This finding may be related to the difficulty of the test, which indicates the presence of mild impairment in working memory in earlier stages. The lack of people’s familiarity with the sequence of the Persian alphabet can be mentioned as a reason justifying this finding. It is suggested that a study with a larger sample size be performed to differentiate the performance of LNST in executive function evaluation from BDST.

Choi et al. (31) observed no difference between normal individuals and IAD in DST (for working memory). Contrary to the results of the present study, in a study by Nigg et al. (32) on adolescents, working memory impairment in IAD patients was sensible. In their work, it was evaluated using Back-2, and the difference in the instrument can well be proposed to justify the difference in the results.

In contrast to our study, a significant difference in DST results was observed between IAD and normal people (Cohen’s d for BDST = 0.6; Cohen’s d for FDST = 0.87). This may be due to the smaller sample size in that study (33). Moreover, in the current work, three instruments were used for the evaluation of executive function (SDMT, LDMT, and VFT), rendering the results more reliable.

In terms of attention and executive function, patients with IAD showed no difference from normal counterparts. Similarly, based on a study by Choi et al., executive function and attention spans were not significantly different from what was observed in the normal group (31). Also, in a study by Park et al. (5), no difference was observed between the normal and IAD groups in terms of executive function. However, working memory and inhibition may be impaired in patients with IAD (34).

In line with other studies (35), we showed that people with ADHD had problems in various cognitive domains. In the ADHD group, there were significant differences in working memory, executive function, and attention span compared to what was observed in the normal group. Similar to our findings, Alderson et al. (36) showed that impairments in working memory continue well into adulthood. It was also revealed that adults with ADHD had deficits in working memory (37). Kim et al. (38) found that neural differences between groups were seen during the working memory tests. In the ADHD group, the DST results were lower than the average range, and the ADHD group had a lower response speed.

Adult people with ADHD have difficulties in many cognitive tests, especially in tasks related to inhibition and working memory (39). Meanwhile, a meta-analysis conducted by Schoechlin et al. (40) showed that unlike what is seen in children with ADHD, the executive function does not generally decrease in afflicted adults. It may be justified that adult patients had better outcomes than children with ADHD based on frontal lobe development with age.

The ADHD group was found to show a significant difference from the IAD group in all three spans of working memory, executive function, and attention, which was predictable due to the similarity between the IAD and normal groups.

Overall, the current results showed that ADHD patients had less performance in attention, executive function, and weaker working memory than IAD and normal people, and working memory was weaker in IAD patients than in normal subjects. Among them, ADHD patients have more significant impairments. In ADHD, cognitive function is more damaged than IAD. This highlights the need for paying more attention to rehabilitation and cognitive interventions in these individuals to prevent such problems as social, academic, and occupational dysfunction.

5.1. Strengths and Limitations

Most patients with IAD also suffer from ADHD at the same time. Given the purpose of the study, a lot of strictness was applied to the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Therefore, the dropout rate increased and the number of samples analyzed was less than expected.

References

  • 1.

    Tranel D, Cooper G, Rodnitzky RL. Higher Brain Functions. Neuroscience in Medicine. 2003. p. 621-39. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-59259-371-2_29.

  • 2.

    Baldacchino A, Balfour DJ, Passetti F, Humphris G, Matthews K. Neuropsychological consequences of chronic opioid use: a quantitative review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(9):2056-68. [PubMed ID: 22771335]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.006.

  • 3.

    Alavi SS, Ferdosi M, Jannatifard F, Eslami M, Alaghemandan H, Setare M. Behavioral addiction versus substance addiction: Correspondence of psychiatric and psychological views. International journal of preventive medicine. 2012;3(4):290.

  • 4.

    Young KS. Caught in the net: How to recognize the signs of internet addiction--and a winning strategy for recovery. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1998.

  • 5.

    Park MH, Park EJ, Choi J, Chai S, Lee JH, Lee C, et al. Preliminary study of Internet addiction and cognitive function in adolescents based on IQ tests. Psychiatry Res. 2011;190(2-3):275-81. [PubMed ID: 21899895]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.08.006.

  • 6.

    Shaw M, Black DW. Internet addiction: definition, assessment, epidemiology and clinical management. CNS Drugs. 2008;22(5):353-65. [PubMed ID: 18399706]. https://doi.org/10.2165/00023210-200822050-00001.

  • 7.

    Cao F, Su L. Internet addiction among Chinese adolescents: prevalence and psychological features. Child Care Health Dev. 2007;33(3):275-81. [PubMed ID: 17439441]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00715.x.

  • 8.

    Babakr ZH, Majeed K, Mohamedamin P, Kakamad K. Internet Addiction in Kurdistan University Students: Prevalence and Association with Self-Control. European Journal of Educational Research. 2019;8(3):867-73. https://doi.org/10.12973/eu-jer.8.3.867.

  • 9.

    Kaltiala-Heino R, Lintonen T, Rimpelä A. Internet addiction? Potentially problematic use of the Internet in a population of 12–18 year-old adolescents. Addiction Research & Theory. 2009;12(1):89-96. https://doi.org/10.1080/1606635031000098796.

  • 10.

    Durkee T, Kaess M, Carli V, Parzer P, Wasserman C, Floderus B, et al. Prevalence of pathological internet use among adolescents in Europe: demographic and social factors. Addiction. 2012;107(12):2210-22. [PubMed ID: 22621402]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03946.x.

  • 11.

    Ghassemzadeh L, Shahraray M, Moradi A. Prevalence of internet addiction and comparison of internet addicts and non-addicts in Iranian high schools. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2008;11(6):731-3. [PubMed ID: 18954277]. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.0243.

  • 12.

    Ansari H, Mohammadpoorasl A, Shahedifar N, Sahebihagh MH, Fakhari A, Hajizadeh M. Internet Addiction and Interpersonal Communication Skills Among High School Students in Tabriz, Iran. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences. 2017;11(2). https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs.4778.

  • 13.

    Biederman J, Faraone S, Milberger S, Curtis S, Chen L, Marrs A, et al. Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence: results from a four-year prospective follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35(3):343-51. [PubMed ID: 8714323]. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199603000-00016.

  • 14.

    Amiri S, Ghoreishizadeh MA, Sadeghi-Bazargani H, Jonggoo M, Golmirzaei J, Abdi S, et al. Prevalence of adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Adult ADHD): Tabriz. Iranian journal of psychiatry. 2014;9(2):83.

  • 15.

    Weiss M, Weiss G. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, A Comprehensive Textbook. Philadelphia: Lippincott William and Wilkins; 2002.

  • 16.

    Yoo HJ, Cho SC, Ha J, Yune SK, Kim SJ, Hwang J, et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms and internet addiction. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;58(5):487-94. [PubMed ID: 15482579]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2004.01290.x.

  • 17.

    Zhou Z, Zhou H, Zhu H. Working memory, executive function and impulsivity in Internet-addictive disorders: a comparison with pathological gambling. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2016;28(2):92-100. [PubMed ID: 26400106]. https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2015.54.

  • 18.

    Hervey AS, Epstein JN, Curry JF. Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(3):485-503. [PubMed ID: 15291727]. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.485.

  • 19.

    Toplak ME, Jain U, Tannock R. Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with Attention-Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Behav Brain Funct. 2005;1(1):8. [PubMed ID: 15982413]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC1183187]. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-1-8.

  • 20.

    Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(3):117-23. [PubMed ID: 16460990]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011.

  • 21.

    Sharifi V, Assadi SM, Mohammadi MR, Amini H, Kaviani H, Semnani Y, et al. A Persian translation of the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition: psychometric properties. Compr Psychiatry. 2009;50(1):86-91. [PubMed ID: 19059520]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.04.004.

  • 22.

    Sadeghi-Bazargani H, Amiri S, Hamraz S, Malek A, Abdi S, Shahrokhi H. Validity and Reliability of the Persian Version of Conner’s Adult ADHD Rating Scales : Observer and Self-report Screening Versions. journal of clinical research & governanace. 2014;3:42-7. https://doi.org/10.13183/jcrg.v3i1.103.

  • 23.

    Ebrahimi A, Molavi H, Mosavi G, Bornamanesh A, Yaghobi M. [Psychometric properties and factor structure of general health questionnaire 28 (GHQ 28) in Iranian psychiatric patients]. Journal of Research in Behavioral Sciences. 2007;5(9):5-12. Persian.

  • 24.

    Taghavi MR. [Validity and reliability of the general health questionnaire (GHQ-28) in college students of Shiraz University]. Journal of Psychology. 2002;5(4):381-98. Persian.

  • 25.

    Rezapour T, Soltani Nezhad Z, Ekhtiari H. [Administration and Scoring Manual Persian Paper and Pencil Cognitive Assessment Package (PCAP)]. Tehran: Mehrsa Publication; 2013. Persian.

  • 26.

    Ghasemian-Shirvan E, Shirazi SM, Aminikhoo M, Zareaan M, Ekhtiari H. Preliminary normative data of Persian phonemic and semantic verbal fluency test. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry. 2018;13(4):288.

  • 27.

    Widyanto L, McMurran M. The psychometric properties of the internet addiction test. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2004;7(4):443-50. [PubMed ID: 15331031]. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2004.7.443.

  • 28.

    Mohammadsalehi N, Mohammadbeigi A, Jadidi R, Anbari Z, Ghaderi E, Akbari M. Psychometric Properties of the Persian Language Version of Yang Internet Addiction Questionnaire: An Explanatory Factor Analysis. Int J High Risk Behav Addict. 2015;4(3). e21560. [PubMed ID: 26495253]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4609498]. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijhrba.21560.

  • 29.

    Wisdom NM, Mignogna J, Collins RL. Variability in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV subtest performance across age. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;27(4):389-97. [PubMed ID: 22512934]. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs041.

  • 30.

    Shelton JT, Elliott EM, Matthews RA, Hill BD, Gouvier WD. The relationships of working memory, secondary memory, and general fluid intelligence: working memory is special. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2010;36(3):813-20. [PubMed ID: 20438278]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC2864949]. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019046.

  • 31.

    Choi JS, Park SM, Roh MS, Lee JY, Park CB, Hwang JY, et al. Dysfunctional inhibitory control and impulsivity in Internet addiction. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(2):424-8. [PubMed ID: 24370334]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.001.

  • 32.

    Nigg JT, Stavro G, Ettenhofer M, Hambrick DZ, Miller T, Henderson JM. Executive functions and ADHD in adults: evidence for selective effects on ADHD symptom domains. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114(4):706-17. [PubMed ID: 16351391]. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.706.

  • 33.

    Zhou Z, Zhu H, Li C, Wang J. Internet Addictive Individuals Share Impulsivity and Executive Dysfunction with Alcohol-Dependent Patients. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00288.

  • 34.

    Nie J, Zhang W, Chen J, Li W. Impaired inhibition and working memory in response to internet-related words among adolescents with internet addiction: A comparison with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2016;236:28-34. [PubMed ID: 26778632]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.004.

  • 35.

    Pievsky MA, McGrath RE. The Neurocognitive Profile of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Review of Meta-Analyses. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2018;33(2):143-57. [PubMed ID: 29106438]. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acx055.

  • 36.

    Alderson RM, Kasper LJ, Hudec KL, Patros CH. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and working memory in adults: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2013;27(3):287-302. [PubMed ID: 23688211]. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032371.

  • 37.

    Schweitzer JB, Hanford RB, Medoff DR. Working memory deficits in adults with ADHD: is there evidence for subtype differences? Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:43. [PubMed ID: 17173676]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC1762010]. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-2-43.

  • 38.

    Kim S, Liu Z, Glizer D, Tannock R, Woltering S. Adult ADHD and working memory: neural evidence of impaired encoding. Clin Neurophysiol. 2014;125(8):1596-603. [PubMed ID: 24411642]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.094.

  • 39.

    Boonstra AM, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA, Buitelaar JK. Executive functioning in adult ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Med. 2005;35(8):1097-108. [PubMed ID: 16116936]. https://doi.org/10.1017/s003329170500499x.

  • 40.

    Schoechlin C, Engel RR. Neuropsychological performance in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of empirical data. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20(6):727-44. [PubMed ID: 15953706]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2005.04.005.