Structural Relationship of Anxious Attachment Style with Pain Catastrophizing, Fear of Pain, and Vigilance to Pain in Non-clinical Pain Cases

authors:

avatar Maryam Amini Fasakhoudi ORCID 1 , avatar Zorieh Abbasi ORCID 2 , * , avatar Abbas Bakhshipour Roudsari ORCID 2 , avatar Maryam Mazaheri ORCID 3

Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Medicine, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education & Psychology, University of Tabriz, Tabriz, Iran
Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Behavioral Science and Mental Health (Tehran Institute of Psychiatry), Iran University of Medical Science, Tehran, Iran

how to cite: Amini Fasakhoudi M, Abbasi Z, Bakhshipour Roudsari A, Mazaheri M. Structural Relationship of Anxious Attachment Style with Pain Catastrophizing, Fear of Pain, and Vigilance to Pain in Non-clinical Pain Cases. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. 2022;16(1):e111215. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs.111215.

Abstract

Background:

The present study aimed to determine the relationship of anxious attachment style with catastrophizing, fear of pain, and hypervigilance.

Methods:

The study sample consisted of 210 students at Tabriz University, Iran, with recent acute pain or the lack of pain experience selected by the convenience sampling method. The subjects responded to the Relationship Scales Questionnaire, Pain Catastrophizing Scale, Fear of Pain Questionnaire, and Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire. Data were analyzed by Pearson’s correlation coefficient and structural equation modeling.

Results:

The findings indicated a significant positive relationship between the subscale of anxious attachment and the variables of catastrophizing, fear of pain, and vigilance to pain, as well as a relationship among the variables. Moreover, the modeling showed a structural relationship between anxious attachment and studied variables.

Conclusions:

Anxious attachment style acts as a vulnerability factor and a predictor of chronic pain in individuals without pain experience. It means that facing catastrophic pain in people with anxious attachment styles can lead to the fear of pain and hypervigilance.

1. Background

Pain is an unpleasant emotional and sensory experience associated with a potential or actual injury that everyone experiences in some way throughout life (1). Pain is experienced in almost everyone at least once in a lifetime, and a small number of individuals with acute pain develop chronic pain (2, 3). Acute pain emerges following tissue damage and should resolve during the healing process. This pain usually lasts for up to three months and, after that, is considered chronic or persistent pain (4). In fact, the experience of acute pain is multidimensional and personalized for each patient and the difference in response to it is associated with biological factors, psychological and non-situational status as well as social context (5).

People may respond differently to pain, and patients who interpret pain as non-traumatic are more likely to be engaged in daily activities and have a greater chance for recovery. In contrast, a vicious cycle develops in those who catastrophize and misinterpret pain (6). Pain catastrophizing is negatively magnifying an actual pain or expected experience. A study showed that catastrophizing, as a mediating variable, causes chronic pain in patients who use it as a defense mechanism (7). According to the fear-avoidance model, pain catastrophizing, as the first variable after experiencing pain, leads to the fear of pain, vigilance to pain, avoidance behaviors, disability, and ultimately more pain perception (8). For example, it has been indicated that catastrophizing and fear of pain are predictors of chronic pain development in injured individuals (9).

Several studies have reported significant relationships between the variables vigilance, fear of pain, and catastrophizing (10, 11). Fear of pain means the severity of a disability, fear of physical activity, and movement due to feeling vulnerable and weak against pain, which plays a prominent role among chronic pain predictors (8). The term vigilance to pain refers to paying too much attention to sensory and physical symptoms, such as pain, and was first used by Chapman in the pain literature (12). The results indicated that the vigilance of people with acute and chronic pain was associated with pain catastrophizing and the fear of pain (13).

Many factors play a role in pain catastrophizing. Some patients catastrophize pain to avoid responsibilities, arouse support, and draw the attention of the important people of their life (14). Empathetic or supportive responses of such individuals may intensify and maintain pain behaviors in these patients (15, 16). For example, patients prolong pain catastrophizing in the presence of their spouses (17). There are various theories about chronic pain and the causes of acute or chronic pain. In recent years, a new line of research claims that the internal working models of attachment theory, as a bio-psychosocial factor, are related to physical health throughout life (18-22).

According to the literature, attachment theory serves as a dynamic model for understanding how pre-existing personality factors, such as an insecure attachment style, can develop chronic pain and disability following acute injury (23). The processes through which insecure attachment may lead to chronic pain and poor judgment about pain are investigated in these studies (24). Wilson and Ruben emphasized the ability of individuals to perceive empathy and support from the important people of their life when facing pain as a function of their attachment pattern (25). Kolb, for the first time, established a relationship between attachment and pain and used attachment theory to express the behavior of patients with pain. This author considered pain-related complaints an attachment behavior formed in childhood through relationships with caregivers and displayed in adulthood relationships. These behaviors include complaining, whining, screaming, clinging, asking about illness, asking for help and support, frequent seeing of a doctor, seeking proximity or isolation, criticizing the support and empathy of others, and denying helplessness (26). There are similarities in reactions to separation from important people in life and pain complaints. The findings suggested that in response to physical pain and separation anxiety, the same mechanisms in the attachment system are activated to regulate negative emotions (27).

According to the Bartholomew model, adult attachment is characterized by two main dimensions anxiety (with a negative view of oneself and a positive view of others) and avoidance (with a negative view of others and a positive view of oneself) (28). Individuals who score high in anxiety are concerned about the accessibility and positive attention of attachment figures (primary caregivers), while those who score high in avoidance feel uncomfortable with closeness and dependence on others (29).

Furthermore, based on a model of attachment styles and emotion regulation strategies, anxious individuals use hyperaction strategies, namely catastrophizing, vigilance, prolonged emotional helplessness, and severe dependence on others. In contrast, individuals with avoidant behavior use deactivation strategies, such as underestimating threats and avoiding the support and empathy of others (30). Hyperaction strategies are characterized by a tendency toward vigilance to a threatening situation, the exaggerated expressions of fears, needs, doubts, and constant concerns about the availability and responsiveness of attachment figures. Applying these strategies indicates high dependence, constant proximity, and attachment to attachment figures. However, deactivation strategies are characterized by ignoring, denying, and suppressing attachment needs. Using the latter approaches reflects the neglect of attachment figures, ignoring their support and empathy, and not being engaged in intimate relationships (31).

In the first study on attachment and pain in a sample without chronic pain, the anxious style was considered a vulnerability factor for chronic pain in patients with acute pain (32). In a study on an individual without chronic pain, people with a preoccupied attachment style (high anxiety and low avoidance) had higher catastrophizing when observing painful figures (33). In another study, the anxious style was identified as a predictor of chronic pain syndrome in patients with acute pain and healthy individuals (34). A study reported a significant relationship between anxious attachment and catastrophizing in a case without chronic pain (35). In the research by McWilliams and Asmundson on 278 students without pain, a significant relationship was found between anxious attachment and variables of pain catastrophizing, the fear of pain, and vigilance to pain. They also found a low correlation between avoidant attachment and catastrophizing, while avoidant attachment and fear of pain and vigilance did not have a relationship (36).

Based on the results of a study, no difference was reported between the two variables of catastrophizing and perception of pain intensity in the two groups with secure and anxious attachment styles in samples without pain (37). Moreover, it has been shown that insecure, anxious attachment separately acts as a mediator of pain and pain behaviors. Therefore, high levels of anxious attachment positively correlate with a poor relationship between pain perception and pain-related behaviors (38).

Some researchers have pointed out that the role of the variables of pain and attachment style in the development of each other cannot be simply stated. In other words, it is difficult to determine the cause-and-effect relationship between the mentioned factors. Research conducted on healthy populations (ie, without pain) can determine pain-related and attachment style variables (39). In fact, by identifying behaviors related to insecure attachment style prior to injury or trauma in subjects without pain and their similarity, the causal relationship of the variables can separately be understood by comparing them with pain behaviors in those with chronic pain and insecure attachment style. Therefore, specialists and clinicians can prevent the development of chronic pain by identifying insecure attachment style in cases with acute tissue injury, as a vulnerable case, by providing necessary training (23).

2. Objectives

With this background in mind, the present study investigated the formation and continuation of the chronic pain cycle in healthy individuals with insecure, anxious attachment styles. According to the literature, no similar study has been conducted in Iran. The hypothetical model of the research is presented in Figure 1.

Conceptual model of the relationships of anxious attachment style with pain catastrophizing, the fear of pain, and vigilance to pain
Conceptual model of the relationships of anxious attachment style with pain catastrophizing, the fear of pain, and vigilance to pain

3. Methods

The current study with the correlational design was performed on a study population of all BA and MA students, as well as the Ph.D. candidates of Tabriz University, Iran. The study samples were selected from physics, chemistry, civil engineering, mechanics, and foreign languages schools by the convenience sampling method. A total of 210 samples (132 females and 78 males) within the age range of 18 - 36 years were enrolled. The investigation aimed to assess the effect of insecure, anxious attachment style on the formation and continuation of the chronic pain cycle in individuals without chronic pain. Consequently, having chronic pain for at least three months was considered the exclusion criterion. The participants were selected through a clinical interview by an expert with a master's degree in clinical psychology, and they completed the research tools under the supervision of the researcher.

3.1. Data Analysis

All the data were analyzed using descriptive statistics (ie, mean and standard deviation), inferential statistics, and Pearson’s correlation coefficient utilizing the SPSS version 21. In addition, LISREL software was applied for structural equation modeling. In data analysis, only completed questionnaires were used.

3.2. Data Collection Tools

3.2.1. Pain Catastrophizing Scale

It was developed by Sullivan, Bishop, and Pivik in 1995. It has 13 items in three subscales of rumination, magnification, and helplessness (40). It is used for clinical and non-clinical populations and has good validity and reliability in students and the clinical population (41). The Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) items are scored on a five-point Likert scale (0 - 4). A study reported the alpha coefficient of 41% of the total variance for rumination, 10% for magnification, and 8% for helplessness (40). Moreover, in research on an Iranian population, Cronbach's alpha coefficient for rumination, magnification, helplessness, and the whole scale was 0.65, 0.53, 0.81, and 0.84, respectively (42).

3.2.2. Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire

It was made by Mc Cracken in 1997 and consisted of 16 items scored based on a six-point Likert scale (0 - 5). It has two subscales of paying attention to pain and paying attention to pain changes. The internal validity of the questionnaire was reported as acceptable with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.87 (43). In the present study, the Cronbach's alpha coefficients of paying attention to pain, paying attention to pain changes, and the whole scale were 0.75, 0.82, and 0.85, respectively.

3.2.3. Fear of Pain Questionnaire

It was designed by McNeil and Rainwater in 1998, consisting of 30 items and scored based on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extreme). It has three subgroups of severe, minor, and medical pain (44). Vala et al. reported the alpha coefficient of Fear of Pain Questionnaire (FPQ) as 0.91 for the whole scale and 0.86, 0.84, and 0.84 for the subscales severe pain, minor pain, and medical pain, respectively (45).

3.2.4. Relationships Scale Questionnaire

It is a continuum measurement tool for determining adult attachment styles and was developed by Griffin and Bartholomew in 1994. Participants respond to it regarding how they establish close emotional relationships with others, and the score of each style is determined as the mean of sum scores for each subscale (46). In a study, the model validity (avoidance continuum) was reported acceptable as 0.68, and the model reliability (anxiety continuum) was 0.5 (47). The validity and reliability of the questionnaire were also examined in Iran. The structural validity of the questionnaire was confirmed by confirmatory factor analysis, and the validity of both anxiety and avoidance models in Cronbach's alpha coefficient was 0.64 and 0.8, respectively. The results showed that the two-factor model fit better than other Relationships Scale Questionnaire (RSQ) scoring models (48). In the present study, a two-factor scoring model was utilized.

4. Results

The demographic data of the study samples are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic Information of the Study Samples

VariablesValues
Gender
Female132
Male78
Level of education
Bachelor’s degree174
Master’s degree36
Marital status
Single182
Married28

The means and standard deviations of the studied variables are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Descriptive Indicators of the Studied Variables

Variable IndexAnxious AttachmentCatastrophizingFear of PainVigilance
Mean ± SD6.95 ± 4.0423.13 ± 9.5766.9 ± 13.9644.06 ± 12
No. 210210210210

The Pearson’s correlation coefficients are given in Table 3.

Table 3.

The Correlation Matrix of the Study Variables

VariablesAnxious AttachmentCatastrophizingFear of PainVigilance
Anxious attachment1
Catastrophizing0.324 a1
Fear of pain0.166 b0.229 a1
Vigilance0.144 b0.372 a0.277 a1

According to Table 3, anxious attachment style had a significant positive relationship with all three variables of pain catastrophizing, the fear of pain, and vigilance to pain. Anxious attachment and catastrophizing had the highest correlation (r = 0.324), which is in line with the theoretical background of the research. In order to clarify the nature of the relationships between the discussed factors, structural equation modeling by LISREL software version 8.8 was used. According to Table 4, the model fit of study samples indicated a good fit with the data, with a chi-square (χ2) of 4361.95, degree of freedom of 1592, and significance level of < 0.001. The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was 0.091, with a range of 0.08 - 0.1, which indicates the average fit of the model. Furthermore, the indices of comparative fit (CFI), normed fit index (NFI), goodness-of-fit (GFI), adjusted GFI (AGFI), and incremental fit (IFI) ranged from 0 to 1, and values closer to 1 show better model fit. According to the data shown in Table 4, the mentioned indices have a moderate fit with the model. The Parsimony NFI (PNFI) and Parsimony GFI (PGFI) are good at the range of 0.5 - 1, and both were in the range, as indicated in Table 4. It can be concluded that the experimental model results, to some extent, supported the theoretical model.

Table 4.

Model Fit Indices

RMSEAχ2dfχ2/dfCFIGFIAGFINFIIFIPNFIPGFI
0.0914361.9515922.730.80.80.850.710.80.680.54

The structural relationship between anxious attachment and pain-related variables is shown in Figure 2. According to Figure 2, all these variables have a significant role in the model.

Structural model of the relationships of anxious attachment style with pain catastrophizing, the fear of pain, and vigilance to pain. The values written on the arrows indicate the amount of variance of the item that can be explained by the agent, and the small arrows show the residual variance (error variance) by Factors are not determined. Chi-square = 4361.95, df = 1592, P-value = 0.00000, RMSEA = 0.091.
Structural model of the relationships of anxious attachment style with pain catastrophizing, the fear of pain, and vigilance to pain. The values written on the arrows indicate the amount of variance of the item that can be explained by the agent, and the small arrows show the residual variance (error variance) by Factors are not determined. Chi-square = 4361.95, df = 1592, P-value = 0.00000, RMSEA = 0.091.

5. Discussion

The results of the present study showed that anxious, insecure attachment style, as a vulnerability factor, plays an effective role in chronic pain. This finding was consistent with those of previous research (32-36) and could be explained in terms of the following probabilities:

According to attachment theory, early interpersonal experiences in attachment figures lead to developing the internal working models of oneself and others in subsequent relationships with important people of life. It helps to regulate emotions when facing stressful situations. One of these stressful conditions is pain, which activates attachment behaviors that bring people closer to attachment figures. These figures help individuals regulate emotions by providing a secure physical and emotional base.

Some researchers believe that both secure and anxious attachment styles similarly absorb the shock against the pain as people with these two styles have a positive view of others and seek support and empathy from attachment figures when needed. However, the difference is that people with secure attachment styles elicit this support easily with appropriate emotion regulation strategies. On the other hand, people with anxious attachment styles seek this support and empathy through inappropriate emotion regulation strategies (ie, hyperaction), such as catastrophizing (37). Unlike people with an avoidant attachment style, who are reluctant to perceive and express their negative emotions, people with an anxious attachment style have a strong tendency toward expressing their emotions because it is compatible with their attachment goals of eliciting support and empathy.

People with an anxious attachment style have low self-esteem due to a negative view of themselves and do not have confidence in their abilities. This feature causes them to be engaged in sticky behaviors. These people lack a secure base because of the experiences they had with their primary caregivers, such as uncertainty in accessibility and receptivity, as well as the use of hyperaction strategies, including vigilance, catastrophizing threats, and generally high sensitivity to the internal and external signs of a threat to gain a secure base (31). People with avoidant attachment styles show less catastrophizing because they have learned from experiences with primary caregivers that if they reveal their helplessness, they are ignored and neglected by their caregivers. At the same time, if they hide the need for support and empathy from their caregivers, they are more likely to receive attention (31).

As stated in the introduction section, according to the fear-avoidance model, people who catastrophize their pain are more likely to exhibit fear of pain, vigilance to pain, and avoidant behaviors that eventually lead to the formation of the vicious cycle of chronic pain (8). According to this model, the fear of pain and vigilance to pain can also be justified in such people. In this regard, the results of the present study, consistent with previous ones, indicated that insecure, anxious attachment style and pain-related variables, such as pain catastrophizing, fear of pain, and hypervigilance, are correlated before the development of chronic pain. Consequently, the potential role of anxious attachment style in developing chronic pain-related mechanisms is justified in individuals without pain.

However, our results were inconsistent with studies demonstrating no difference between the two variables of pain catastrophizing and the perception of pain intensity in individuals with secure and anxious attachment styles. In other words, both safe and anxious styles reported high tolerance thresholds for painful stimuli (37). This unexpected finding may be attributed to people with secure and anxious attachment styles having positive thoughts and feelings toward others and expecting the accessibility and support of their primary caregivers when they face pain. In people with a secure attachment style, this expectation is positive, while negative in those with an anxious attachment style due to ambivalent feelings. Despite ambivalent feelings, a higher tolerance threshold of people with an anxious attachment style can be justified by the social pain theory. In fact, according to this theory developed by Macdoland and Leary, people with anxious attachment styles, not receiving support from their primary caregivers, respond to pain by reducing sensitivity. Therefore, analgesia is an adaptive response in such individuals (49). The Vlaeyen and Linton model and the results of most previous research revealed a significant relationship between the variables catastrophizing, fear of pain, and vigilance to pain (8).

Considering the mentioned debates and based on the attachment theory and Bartholomew model of anxious and avoidance styles, as well as the model for emotion regulation strategies, people with insecure style cannot use appropriate emotion regulation strategies due to the lack of developed behavioral coping and emotion regulation strategies when facing stressors, such as pain. As a result, they become helpless over time, cannot trust their secure figures, and establish intimate relationships with them, leading to catastrophizing. According to the Vlaeyen and Linton model, these catastrophic thoughts, in turn, result in the idea of fear of pain and, consequently, more vigilance to pain. The activation of this mechanism elicits support and empathy from people who are important in life in the short term, while it works adversely in the long run and even may lead to rejection by them. Consequently, intensified catastrophizing and the chronic pain cycle may occur. These explanations show why pain in people with an anxious attachment style is chronic.

Assessing fixed (dispositional) personality traits in the early stages of pain experience and giving insights to individuals can prevent the development of complex mechanisms involved in the chronic pain process. According to the social pain theory and the role of external factors, such as the support and empathy of others, the importance of internal factors, such as attachment styles, should also be highlighted (49).

5.1. Limitations and Suggestions

The sample of the current investigation may not represent the whole population due to the employment of the convenience sampling method. Therefore, the results should be generalized with caution. Another limitation of our study was the disproportion of female participants to male subjects, which should be considered in future studies. Given the high prevalence of chronic pain and the drastic costs of recovery imposed on both individuals and society, preventive methods for people at the risk of chronic pain, identification of risk factors, and striving for early intervention are recommended. It was the first study on this subject in Iran. As a result, further research is recommended to better identify the potential role of attachment styles and choose the appropriate treatment for each.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Raja SN, Carr DB, Cohen M, Finnerup NB, Flor H, Gibson S, et al. The revised International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises. Pain. 2020;161(9):1976-82. [PubMed ID: 32694387]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7680716]. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001939.

  • 2.

    Blyth FM, Jones GT, Macfarlane GJ. Musculoskeletal health--how early does it start? Rheumatology (Oxford). 2009;48(10):1181-2. [PubMed ID: 19620206]. https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/kep213.

  • 3.

    Goubert L, Crombez G, De Bourdeaudhuij I. Low back pain, disability and back pain myths in a community sample: prevalence and interrelationships. Eur J Pain. 2004;8(4):385-94. [PubMed ID: 15207520]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2003.11.004.

  • 4.

    Schug SA, Lavand'homme P, Barke A, Korwisi B, Rief W, Treede RD, et al. The IASP classification of chronic pain for ICD-11: chronic postsurgical or posttraumatic pain. Pain. 2019;160(1):45-52. [PubMed ID: 30586070]. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001413.

  • 5.

    Small C, Laycock H. Acute postoperative pain management. Br J Surg. 2020;107(2):e70-80. [PubMed ID: 31903595]. https://doi.org/10.1002/bjs.11477.

  • 6.

    Meulders A, Vansteenwegen D, Vlaeyen JWS. The acquisition of fear of movement-related pain and associative learning: a novel pain-relevant human fear conditioning paradigm. Pain. 2011;152(11):2460-9. [PubMed ID: 21723664]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.05.015.

  • 7.

    Kremer R, Granot M, Yarnitsky D, Crispel Y, Fadel S, Anson Best L, et al. The Role of Pain Catastrophizing in the Prediction of Acute and Chronic Postoperative Pain. Open Pain J. 2013;6(1):176-82. https://doi.org/10.2174/1876386301306010176.

  • 8.

    Vlaeyen JW, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain. 2000;85(3):317-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00242-0.

  • 9.

    Niederstrasser NG, Meulders A, Meulders M, Slepian PM, Vlaeyen JW, Sullivan MJ. Pain Catastrophizing and Fear of Pain Predict the Experience of Pain in Body Parts Not Targeted by a Delayed-Onset Muscle Soreness Procedure. J Pain. 2015;16(11):1065-76. [PubMed ID: 26277643]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2015.07.008.

  • 10.

    Roelofs J, Peters ML, Muris P, Vlaeyen JWS. Dutch version of the Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire: validity and reliability in a pain-free population. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40(9):1081-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00008-6.

  • 11.

    Roelofs J, Peters ML, Vlaeyen JW. The modified Stroop paradigm as a measure of selective attention towards pain-related information in patients with chronic low back pain. Psychol Rep. 2003;92(3 Pt 1):707-15. [PubMed ID: 12841430]. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2003.92.3.707.

  • 12.

    Chapman CR. Pain: The perception of noxious events. The psychology of pain. New York, USA: Raven Press; 1978. p. 169-202.

  • 13.

    Goubert L, Crombez G, Van Damme S. The role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear in vigilance to pain: a structural equations approach. Pain. 2004;107(3):234-41. [PubMed ID: 14736586]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2003.11.005.

  • 14.

    Sullivan MJ, Tripp DA, Santor D. Gender differences in pain and pain behavior: the role of catastrophizing. Cognit Ther Res. 2000;24(1):121-34. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005459110063.

  • 15.

    Goubert L, Craig KD, Vervoort T, Morley S, Sullivan MJL, Williams CAC, et al. Facing others in pain: the effects of empathy. Pain. 2005;118(3):285-8. [PubMed ID: 16289804]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2005.10.025.

  • 16.

    Cano A, Leonard MT, Franz A. The significant other version of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS-S): preliminary validation. Pain. 2005;119(1-3):26-37. [PubMed ID: 16298062]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC2679670]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2005.09.009.

  • 17.

    Sullivan MJL, Adams H, Sullivan ME. Communicative dimensions of pain catastrophizing: social cueing effects on pain behaviour and coping. Pain. 2004;107(3):220-6. [PubMed ID: 14736584]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2003.11.003.

  • 18.

    Bernard K, Hostinar CE, Dozier M. Longitudinal associations between attachment quality in infancy, C-reactive protein in early childhood, and BMI in middle childhood: preliminary evidence from a CPS-referred sample. Attach Hum Dev. 2019;21(1):5-22. [PubMed ID: 30406720]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC8815256]. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1541513.

  • 19.

    Ehrlich KB, Miller GE, Jones JD, Cassidy J. Attachment & Psychoneuroimmunology. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, reaserch, and clinical applications. 3rd ed. New York, USA: Guilford Press; 2016. p. 180-201.

  • 20.

    Farrell AK, Waters TEA, Young ES, Englund MM, Carlson EE, Roisman GI, et al. Early maternal sensitivity, attachment security in young adulthood, and cardiometabolic risk at midlife. Attach Hum Dev. 2019;21(1):70-86. [PubMed ID: 30428778]. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1541517.

  • 21.

    Gouin JP, MacNeil S. Attachment style and changes in systemic inflammation following migration to a new country among international students. Attach Hum Dev. 2019;21(1):38-56. [PubMed ID: 30406717]. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1541515.

  • 22.

    Harvey MW, Farrell AK, Imami L, Carre JM, Slatcher RB. Maternal attachment avoidance is linked to youth diurnal cortisol slopes in children with asthma. Attach Hum Dev. 2019;21(1):23-37. [PubMed ID: 30406724]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6413731]. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1541514.

  • 23.

    Andersen TE, Sterling M, Maujean A, Meredith P. Attachment insecurity as a vulnerability factor in the development of chronic whiplash associated disorder - A prospective cohort study. J Psychosom Res. 2019;118:56-62. [PubMed ID: 30782355]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2019.01.008.

  • 24.

    Meredith P, Ownsworth T, Strong J. A review of the evidence linking adult attachment theory and chronic pain: presenting a conceptual model. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008;28(3):407-29. [PubMed ID: 17719157]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.009.

  • 25.

    Wilson CL, Ruben MA. A pain in her arm: Romantic attachment orientations and the tourniquet task. Pers Relat. 2011;18(2):242-65. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01359.x.

  • 26.

    Kolb LC. Attachment behavior and pain complaints. Psychosomatics. 1982;23(4):413-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0033-3182(82)73404-8.

  • 27.

    Walsh TM, Symons DK, McGrath PJ. Relations between young children's responses to the depiction of separation and pain experiences. Attach Hum Dev. 2004;6(1):53-71. [PubMed ID: 14982679]. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730410001663489.

  • 28.

    Bartholomew K. Avoidance of Intimacy: An Attachment Perspective. J Soc Pers Relat. 1990;7(2):147-78. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407590072001.

  • 29.

    Bartholomew K, Horowitz LM. Attachment styles among young adults: a test of a four-category model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61(2):226-44. [PubMed ID: 1920064]. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.61.2.226.

  • 30.

    Mikulincer M, Shaver PR, Pereg D. Attachment theory and affect regulation: the dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attachment-related strategies. Motiv Emot. 2003;27(2):77-102. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1024515519160.

  • 31.

    Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Adult attachment and affect regulation. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Structure, Dynamics, and change. New York, USA: Guilford Press; 2007. p. 503-33.

  • 32.

    Meredith PJ, Strong J, Feeney JA. The relationship of adult attachment to emotion, catastrophizing, control, threshold and tolerance, in experimentally-induced pain. Pain. 2006;120(1-2):44-52. [PubMed ID: 16359795]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2005.10.008.

  • 33.

    Pan Y, Zhang D, Liu Y, Ran G, Teng Z. The effects of attachment style and security priming on the perception of others’ pain. J Soc Pers Relat. 2016;34(2):184-208. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407515627509.

  • 34.

    Meredith PJ, Strong J, Feeney JA. Evidence of a relationship between adult attachment variables and appraisals of chronic pain. Pain Res Manag. 2005;10(4):191-200. [PubMed ID: 16341306]. https://doi.org/10.1155/2005/745650.

  • 35.

    McWilliams LA, Holmberg D. Adult attachment and pain catastrophizing for self and significant other. Pain. 2010;149(2):278-83. [PubMed ID: 20207079]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.02.019.

  • 36.

    McWilliams LA, Asmundson GJ. The relationship of adult attachment dimensions to pain-related fear, hypervigilance, and catastrophizing. Pain. 2007;127(1-2):27-34. [PubMed ID: 16963183]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.07.020.

  • 37.

    Rowe AC, Carnelley KB, Harwood J, Micklewright D, Russouw L, Rennie CL, et al. The effect of attachment orientation priming on pain sensitivity in pain-free individuals. J Soc Pers Relat. 2012;29(4):488-507. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407511431189.

  • 38.

    Andersen TE, Ravn SL, Manniche C, O'Neill S. The impact of attachment insecurity on pain and pain behaviors in experimental pain. J Psychosom Res. 2018;111:127-32. [PubMed ID: 29935745]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.06.002.

  • 39.

    Meredith PJ. A review of the evidence regarding associations between attachment theory and experimentally induced pain. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2013;17(4):326-34. [PubMed ID: 23456784]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11916-013-0326-y.

  • 40.

    Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation. Psychol Assess. 1995;7(4):524-32. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524.

  • 41.

    Van Damme S, Crombez G, Bijttebier P, Goubert L, Van Houdenhove B. A confirmatory factor analysis of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale: invariant factor structure across clinical and non-clinical populations. Pain. 2002;96(3):319-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00463-8.

  • 42.

    Mohamadi S, Dehghani M, Heidari M, Sedaghat M, Khatibi A. The evaluation of pain-related psychological similarities among patients with musculoskeletal chronic pain and their spouses. Int J Behav Sci. 2013;7(1):57-66.

  • 43.

    McCracken LM. “Attention” to pain in persons with chronic pain: A behavioral approach. Behavior Therapy. 1997;28(2):271-84. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7894(97)80047-0.

  • 44.

    McNeil DW, Rainwater A3. Development of the Fear of Pain Questionnaire--III. J Behav Med. 1998;21(4):389-410. [PubMed ID: 9789168]. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1018782831217.

  • 45.

    Vala M, Dehghani M, Kiamanesh AR. [Validity, reliability and normalization of the fear of pain questionnaire-short form [FPQ-SF]]. J Kerman University Med Sci. 2012;19(2):176-89. Persian.

  • 46.

    Griffin DW, Bartholomew K. Models of the self and other: Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67(3):430-45. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.430.

  • 47.

    Backstrom M, Holmes BM. Measuring adult attachment: a construct validation of two self-report instruments. Scand J Psychol. 2001;42(1):79-86. [PubMed ID: 11273581]. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00216.

  • 48.

    Amini Fasakhoudi M, Bakhshipour Roudsari A. Investigating Reliability, Validity and Factor Structure of the Persian Version of the Relationship Scales Questionnaire. J Fam Res. 2015;11(3):333-52.

  • 49.

    Macdonald G, Leary MR. Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychol Bull. 2005;131(2):202-23. [PubMed ID: 15740417]. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202.