The Impact of Table Tennis Exercises on Working Memory of Educable Intellectually Disabled Students

authors:

avatar Amir Hamzeh Sabzi 1 , * , avatar Azar Aghayari 1 , avatar Zahra Ramrodi 1

Department of Physical Education, Payam Noor University, Tehran, Iran

how to cite: Sabzi A H, Aghayari A, Ramrodi Z. The Impact of Table Tennis Exercises on Working Memory of Educable Intellectually Disabled Students. J Motor Control Learn. 2023;5(4):e146342. https://doi.org/10.5812/jmcl-146342.

Abstract

Background:

This study aimed to investigate the impact of table tennis exercises on the working memory of educable intellectually disabled students.

Methods:

Thirty intellectually disabled students aged 9 - 15 in Galikesh City were randomly selected and assigned into 2 groups: An experimental group and a control group. The experimental group received table tennis exercises for 8 weeks (3 sessions per week), each lasting 60 minutes. The control group engaged in regular daily activities. Working memory was assessed using the Daneman and Carpenter's Working Memory Test. Data analysis was conducted using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

Results:

The results of ANCOVA indicated a significant difference in the mean of working memory performance between the 2 groups after the intervention (P < 0.05). Thus, table tennis exercises had a significant effect on improving the working memory of intellectually disabled students.

Conclusions:

Table tennis exercises are effective in enhancing the working memory of intellectually disabled students and can be used as an appropriate intervention.

1. Background

Intellectually disabled children have general intelligence scores lower than average (IQ scores of 70 or lower), including approximately 2% to 3% of individuals in any given society (1). These children are classified under the diagnostic category of neurodevelopmental disorders, encompassing impairments in cognitive, conceptual skills, social skills, and practical skills (2). Intellectually disabled children struggle with retaining and remembering information. Their primary challenges lie in their working memory and long-term memory, and the more abstract the information, the greater their difficulties will be (3). Working memory is the ability to retain information within the mind while performing complex tasks. It includes the ability to monitor and assess cognitive processes and serves as the mental system responsible for temporarily storing and processing information to accomplish a series of complex cognitive tasks (4). The formation and maintenance of memory are associated with encoding, consolidation, retrieval, and reconsolidation processes (5). On the other hand, memory is considered one of the indicators of intelligence; however, in reality, it is the primary foundation of intelligence (6). Today, working memory is a crucial subject that receives considerable attention and has captivated the interest of many researchers (7). Memory, as a cognitive function, progresses during childhood development. Children spend more time processing cognitive information and recalling it than adults. It seems that factors like the speed of memory-related processes (encoding, consolidation, and retrieval) are responsible for the processing speed of information in children rather than environmental factors (8). on the other hand, the consolidation of memory is a time-dependent process in which motor memory becomes more stable over time. From a neurobiological perspective, during the learning and practice of a motor skill, encoding and memory consolidation processes take shape. With more practice and repetition, the reconsolidation process becomes stronger. In other words, newly acquired memory is initially influenced by consolidation mechanisms and later becomes engaged in reconsolidation mechanisms (9). Research results suggest that motor skill interventions may improve executive functions (8, 10). For example, Sheikh et al. demonstrated that Spark exercises improved the visual-spatial working memory of intellectually disabled children (11). Additionally, Jalil-Abkenar et al. found that the Arsh leisure activity program enhanced the working memory of intellectually disabled students (12). Sports can be categorized into various types based on factors like training methods and playing styles. Voss et al. classify sports into static, tracking, and strategic categories (13). Considering the hypothesis of broad skill transfer, different cognitive demands of various sports and the accumulation of experience through their evident practice should lead to differences in cognitive performance. According to this classification, study findings indicate that strategic athletes exhibit better executive performance (e.g., inhibition and switching) compared to static athletes (14). Furthermore, strategic athletes likely have higher cognitive demands in visual-spatial working memory since they process more visual-spatial information (teammates, opponents, field positions, and the ball) compared to other types of sports (15). For example, Crova et al. showed that tennis exercises, compared to aerobic exercises, led to greater improvements in working memory and inhibitory response in overweight children (16). Additionally, in another study, due to higher cognitive loads, team games significantly improved cognitive flexibility in children compared to aerobic exercises (17).

Table tennis is a technical and tactical sport that requires a complex combination of physical abilities, such as strength, power, speed, agility, aerobic and anaerobic capacity, and neuromuscular coordination (18). In table tennis, players repeatedly hit a ball on a relatively small table, controlling its spin, speed, and placement to make it difficult for the opponent to return successfully (19). That is, for effective performance, a player must predict the opponent's shot, recognize meaningful cues in the game, make split-second decisions about which action to take, and manage the appropriate response within serious time constraints (20). Mistakes in returning the ball result in the opponent scoring a point in a rally, making victory in the game more challenging. Given these constraints, it is logical to assume that a higher level of cognition is crucial for successful performance in table tennis (21). Considering that there have been few studies on the role of table tennis in improving the working memory of educable intellectually disabled children, there is a clear lack of empirical research on the impact of table tennis on the working memory of such students.

The current research seeks to answer the question of whether table tennis exercises improve the working memory of intellectually disabled children.

2. Methods

2.1. Subjects

The research design employed in this study was a semi-experimental design, incorporating a pretest, posttest, and a control group. The statistical population consisted of all educable intellectually disabled female students in Galikesh County. A convenience sampling method was used to select a sample of 30 students between the ages of 9 and 15. These participants were then randomly assigned to either the experimental or control group, with each group consisting of 15 participants. The sample size for each group was determined by taking into account a confidence level of 95%, a statistical power of 0.8, and the observed standard deviation obtained from previous research. The formula proposed by Cohen (1970) was used for sample size calculation, resulting in 15 participants assigned to each group. The design of this research has been approved by the Ethics Committee of Payam Noor University.

2.2. Apparatus and Task

Daneman and Carpenter's Working Memory Test: This test consists of 27 sentences. These 27 sentences are divided into 6 sections, with 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, and 7 sentences in each section, respectively (22). In each section, the participants are required to listen to the sentences, which are relatively challenging and unrelated to each other, and then perform 2 tasks: Determining whether the sentence is semantically correct or not and recalling the last word of each sentence. The first part of the test assesses processing capacity, while the second part assesses retention. To score the Working Memory Test, the number of correct answers in each section is divided by the total number of sentences, and then the scores from each section are summed and divided by 2. This test has high validity; thus, its correlation with the test of real questions is 0.72, and the correlation with the test of pointed pronouns is 0.9. Regarding the reliability of this test, a reliability coefficient of 0.85 was obtained in a study conducted on 119 third-grade middle school students (22).

2.3. Procedure

In the implementation phase, the necessary correspondence and coordination were initially conducted to introduce the researcher to the exceptional schools in Galikesh. After determining the participants, detailed explanations about the research process and the benefits of participation were provided, and written consent was obtained from the parents of the students. Participants were assured of their ethical considerations and informed that they could withdraw from the study at any time, and all information would be kept confidential. Afterward, based on the coordinated efforts, a specific schedule was set for conducting the tests and participating in the intervention program. The data collection process involved conducting a pretest of working memory for both groups; then, the participants were randomly assigned to the experimental and control groups. The experimental group engaged in table tennis exercises for 8 weeks (3 sessions per week), each lasting 60 minutes. During this period, the control group only engaged in their daily activities. The training program for the table tennis group was designed in accordance with studies conducted by Pan et al. (23) and Tsai (24). The table tennis sessions included warm-up (5 minutes), teaching basic table tennis skills and advanced skills (25 minutes), group games and table tennis situations (25 minutes), and cooling down (5 minutes). Initially, basic table tennis skills were gradually taught, including ball and racket handling, basic forehand and backhand strokes, forehand and backhand practice rallies, serving (forehand and backhand serves), and returning the coach's strokes, as well as proper foot movement and positioning (e.g., ready position, 1-legged footwork, 2-legged footwork, and cross-step footwork). Afterward, participants engaged in designed games to practice their table tennis skills. After the last session, a posttest of working memory was administered to all participants, and the results were recorded for further analysis.

2.4. Data Analysis

In the data analysis, the Shapiro-Wilk test was used to assess the normality of data distribution, and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) tests were applied. All analyses were conducted with a 95% confidence level using SPSS version 22.

3. Results

Table 1 displays the descriptive statistics for the research variables and provides a summary of the results of ANCOVA to analyze the effect of independent variables on dependent variables. To conduct between-group comparisons, the assumptions required for using ANCOVA (normality of data distribution, homogeneity of variances, linearity of the relationship between the dependent variable and covariates, and homogeneity of regression slopes) were first examined. The results of the Shapiro-Wilk test showed that the data distribution related to working memory is normally distributed. The Levene's test results indicated that the variances of working memory data in both the pre- and posttest are homogeneous. Additionally, the scatter plots of the variables demonstrated a linear relationship between the covariates and dependent variables. To examine the homogeneity of regression slopes, ANCOVA was conducted to assess the lack of a statistically significant interaction between independent variables (group) and covariates (pretest levels). The results showed that the interaction effect of the group and the covariate on working memory is not statistically significant. Given the fulfillment of assumptions, ANCOVA was employed for between-group comparisons, with the (experimental/control) group variable as the independent variable, working memory levels as the dependent variable, and pretest values as the control variable (covariate). The research findings regarding working memory indicated that after controlling for the pretest effect, the group effect on working memory was statistically significant (P < 0.001; F = 0.07). The mean values of the groups in the posttest suggest that the working memory performance in the experimental group is significantly higher than that of the control group (Table 1).

Table 1.

The Results of ANCOVA

VariablesStagesMean ± SDFPη2
Experimental GroupControl Group
Working memoryPre-test61.70 ± 5.861.80 ± 7.6531.5800.1000.326

4. Discussion

The aim of this research was to investigate the effect of table tennis exercise on the working memory of educable intellectually disabled students. Motor experiences and physical activities are known to enhance cognitive processes. In other words, based on Piaget's theory, exploration of the environment and interaction with the surrounding environment can impact cognitive processes (25). Accordingly, the results obtained in this study demonstrated that table tennis exercises have a significant impact on the working memory of students. These findings indicate that table tennis exercises have a positive effect on students' working memory. Consistent with the findings of the present study, various studies using different types of physical activities have shown the effectiveness of exercise programs in improving working memory. These include the effects of gymnastic exercises (26), active games (27), skill-based exercises (28), aerobic exercises (29), selected games (30), and spark exercises (11). Previous studies have shown that due to their different cognitive requirements, different sports can create differences in the transfer of cognitive skills, including working memory function (15). Among various sports, strategic disciplines require more cognitive skills. Table tennis is considered one of these strategic disciplines in which individuals must simultaneously process information about teammates, opponents, court and ball positions, and often a variety of situations. Furthermore, cognitive task training and exercises can improve cognitive performance (31, 32). Therefore, the improvement in working memory due to table tennis exercises can be attributed to the enhancement of participants' cognitive skills.

The results of some studies are not consistent with the findings of the current research. For example, certain studies have demonstrated that exercises focusing on closed motor skills do not have a significant impact on working memory (33, 34). The lack of effect observed in this type of skill exercise on working memory can be attributed to the nature of these skills. Closed motor skills involve a relatively stable environment, and the performer pre-selects and organizes movements without being under time pressure. They execute these movements without the need for modifications (28). This factor makes it easier for the performer to engage in stimulus selection and response selection processes, resulting in lower cognitive processing demands on working memory. In line with the findings of the current research, a significant body of research indicates that physical activity induces specific changes in the functioning of the nervous system and enhances processes related to learning and memory. It is highly likely that these effects can be attributed to changes occurring in hippocampal neuroplasticity, such as neuron proliferation, long-term potentiation, and particularly an increase in brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) resulting from participation in physical activities (35). BDNF, a neurotrophin abundantly present in the brain, exhibits high activity levels in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, both of which are crucial areas for learning, memory, and cognitive processes. Additionally, BDNF plays a significant role in brain growth, synapse formation, regulation of neurotransmitters, and enhancing learning and memory processes while also providing protection for nerve cells against neurodegeneration. A deficiency in BDNF impairs long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. However, this synaptic dysfunction can be rectified through the upregulation or expression of high levels of BDNF (35). BDNF also plays a key role in the survival of midbrain dopamine neurons. Moreover, it acts as a noteworthy neurotransmitter in regulating attention and cognitive performance (36).

On the other hand, evidence suggests that there may be a potential interaction between exercise, developmental stages, and the medial temporal lobe memory system (35). In general, significant research indicates that behavioral manipulation during critical stages of life can have long-term and impactful consequences. The persistence of physical activity during childhood and adolescence, a period characterized by heightened neural plasticity, may lead to long-term alterations contrary to its transitory effects in adulthood. These consequences may be associated with significant functional and morphological reorganization observed in childhood and adolescent brain regions, such as the medial temporal lobe and the hippocampus, playing a role in learning and memory. Exercise stimulates neural plasticity and cortical pathways while simultaneously reducing the learning threshold in brain regions involved in cognitive experiences (35).

Like any study, the present research faced limitations that necessitate caution when interpreting and generalizing the results. Due to logistical challenges, the study employed convenience sampling to ensure a more accurate generalization of results to the research population.

4.1. Conclusions

Moreover, the results of this research are generalizable to intellectually disabled students aged 9 to 15 in Galikesh County, and if necessary, extending these findings to other intellectually disabled students should be done cautiously and with sufficient knowledge. Considering the theoretical framework, the findings, and the existing limitations, it is recommended that this research be conducted in other geographical regions and diverse cultural backgrounds, with a larger sample size, if possible. Additionally, neuroimaging techniques should be employed for a more in-depth investigation of the impact of different types of sports on memory.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Luckasson R, Borthwick-Duffy S, Buntinx WH, Coulter DL, Craig EMP, Reeve A, et al. Mental retardation: Definition, classification, and systems of supports. American Association on Mental Retardation; 2002.

  • 2.

    Ghasemzadeh S, Naghdi N, Afrooz G. [The Effect of Games and Activities Based on Sher's Project on Fine and Gross Motor Skills in Slow-Paced Children]. J Sports Motor Develop Learning. 2017;9(3):445-56. Persian.

  • 3.

    Lifshitz H, Kilberg E, Vakil E. Working memory studies among individuals with intellectual disability: An integrative research review. Res Dev Disabil. 2016;59:147-65. [PubMed ID: 27614274]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2016.08.001.

  • 4.

    Brooks SJ, Funk SG, Young SY, Schioth HB. The Role of Working Memory for Cognitive Control in Anorexia Nervosa versus Substance Use Disorder. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1651. [PubMed ID: 29018381]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5615794]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01651.

  • 5.

    Kantak SS, Winstein CJ. Learning-performance distinction and memory processes for motor skills: a focused review and perspective. Behav Brain Res. 2012;228(1):219-31. [PubMed ID: 22142953]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.028.

  • 6.

    Esbjørn BH, Normann N, Christiansen BM, Reinholdt-Dunne ML. The efficacy of group metacognitive therapy for children (MCT-c) with generalized anxiety disorder: An open trial. J Anxiety Disord. 2018;53:16-21. eng. [PubMed ID: 29145078]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.11.002.

  • 7.

    Dagenais E, Rouleau I, Tremblay A, Demers M, Roger E, Jobin C, et al. Role of executive functions in prospective memory in multiple sclerosis: Impact of the strength of cue-action association. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2016;38(1):127-40. [PubMed ID: 26588195]. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1091063.

  • 8.

    Hillman CPMCD, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudde RMDE, Moore RD, Wu CT, et al. Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics. 2021. [PubMed ID: 25266425]. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3219.

  • 9.

    Nader K. Reconsolidation and the Dynamic Nature of Memory. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2015;7(10). a021782. [PubMed ID: 26354895]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4588064]. https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021782.

  • 10.

    Saadat M. Which symptoms of ADHD would improve with play therapy and yoga? Journal of Exceptional Children. 2011;11(1):45-55.

  • 11.

    Sheikh M, Ramezani S, Maghsoudy G, Ahmadi K. The Effects of Spark Training on Visual-Spatial Working Memory Operation in Children with Mental Retardation. The Neuroscience Journal of Shefaye Khatam. 2019;7(3):13-22. https://doi.org/10.29252/shefa.7.3.13.

  • 12.

    Jalil-Abkenar SS, Afrooz GA, Arjmandnia AA, Ghobari-Bonab B. [the effectiveness of arsh leisure time program on the profile working memory of children with educatable intellectual disability]. Arch Rehabilitation. 2019;20(1). Persian.

  • 13.

    Voss MW, Kramer AF, Basak C, Prakash RS, Roberts B. Are expert athletes ‘expert’ in the cognitive laboratory? A meta‐analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2009;24(6):812-26. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1588.

  • 14.

    Krenn B, Finkenzeller T, Würth S, Amesberger G. Sport type determines differences in executive functions in elite athletes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2018;38:72-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.06.002.

  • 15.

    Sato T, Kosaki K, Choi Y, Tochigi Y, Shindo-Hamasaki A, Momma R, et al. Association between sport types and visuospatial working memory in athletes. The Journal of Physical Fitness and Sports Medicine. 2022;11(4):247-53. https://doi.org/10.7600/jpfsm.11.247.

  • 16.

    Crova C, Struzzolino I, Marchetti R, Masci I, Vannozzi G, Forte R, et al. Cognitively challenging physical activity benefits executive function in overweight children. J Sports Sci. 2014;32(3):201-11. [PubMed ID: 24015968]. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.828849.

  • 17.

    Schmidt M, Jager K, Egger F, Roebers CM, Conzelmann A. Cognitively Engaging Chronic Physical Activity, But Not Aerobic Exercise, Affects Executive Functions in Primary School Children: A Group-Randomized Controlled Trial. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2015;37(6):575-91. [PubMed ID: 26866766]. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2015-0069.

  • 18.

    Zagatto AM, Kondric M, Knechtle B, Nikolaidis PT, Sperlich B. Energetic demand and physical conditioning of table tennis players. A study review. J Sports Sci. 2018;36(7):724-31. [PubMed ID: 28582628]. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2017.1335957.

  • 19.

    Tamaki S, Yoshida K, Yamada K. A Shot Number Based Approach to Performance Analysis in Table Tennis. J Hum Kinet. 2017;55:7-18. [PubMed ID: 28210334]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5304271]. https://doi.org/10.1515/hukin-2017-0002.

  • 20.

    Abernethy B. Visual search strategies and decision-making in sport. International journal of sport psychology. 1991.

  • 21.

    Elferink-Gemser MT, Faber IR, Visscher C, Hung TM, de Vries SJ, Nijhuis-Van der Sanden MWG. Higher-level cognitive functions in Dutch elite and sub-elite table tennis players. PLoS One. 2018;13(11). e0206151. eng. [PubMed ID: 30403711]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6221298]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206151.

  • 22.

    Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 1980;19(4):450-66.

  • 23.

    Pan CY, Tsai CL, Chu CH, Sung MC, Huang CY, Ma WY. Effects of Physical Exercise Intervention on Motor Skills and Executive Functions in Children With ADHD: A Pilot Study. J Atten Disord. 2019;23(4):384-97. [PubMed ID: 25646023]. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054715569282.

  • 24.

    Tsai CL. The effectiveness of exercise intervention on inhibitory control in children with developmental coordination disorder: using a visuospatial attention paradigm as a model. Res Dev Disabil. 2009;30(6):1268-80. [PubMed ID: 19497707]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2009.05.001.

  • 25.

    Ghorbanzadeh B, Lotfi M. [Effect of selected perceptual-motor practices on memory and attention problems in children with intellectual disability]. Sports Psychol Studies. 2015;14:45-58. Persian.

  • 26.

    Serra L, Raimondi S, di Domenico C, Maffei S, Lardone A, Liparoti M, et al. The beneficial effects of physical exercise on visuospatial working memory in preadolescent children. AIMS Neurosci. 2021;8(4):496-509. [PubMed ID: 34877401]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC8611191]. https://doi.org/10.3934/Neuroscience.2021026.

  • 27.

    Akbarifar H, Sharifidaramadi P, Rahimzade H, Pezeshk S. The Effectiveness of the Intervention of Active Plays on Working Memory in Students with Dyslexia. Neuropsychology. 2019;5(16):149-62.

  • 28.

    Jalilvand M, Samadi H, Dana A. [effectiveness of physical activity course on preschool children’s working memory: Emphasizing environmental change]. Sci J Rehabilitation Med. 2021;10(5):936-47. Persian.

  • 29.

    Chaire A, Becke A, Duzel E. Effects of Physical Exercise on Working Memory and Attention-Related Neural Oscillations. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:239. [PubMed ID: 32296302]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7136837]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2020.00239.

  • 30.

    Zhang JY, Shen QQ, Wang DL, Hou JM, Xia T, Qiu S, et al. Physical activity intervention promotes working memory and motor competence in preschool children. Front Public Health. 2022;10:984887. [PubMed ID: 36225790]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC9549139]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2022.984887.

  • 31.

    Jacobson J, Matthaeus L. Athletics and executive functioning: How athletic participation and sport type correlate with cognitive performance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2014;15(5):521-7.

  • 32.

    Wang CH, Chang CC, Liang YM, Shih CM, Chiu WS, Tseng P, et al. Open vs. closed skill sports and the modulation of inhibitory control. PLoS One. 2013;8(2). e55773. [PubMed ID: 23418458]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3572130]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055773.

  • 33.

    Koch P, Krenn B. Executive functions in elite athletes – Comparing open-skill and closed-skill sports and considering the role of athletes' past involvement in both sport categories. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2021;55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2021.101925.

  • 34.

    Yu Q, Chan CCH, Chau B, Fu ASN. Motor skill experience modulates executive control for task switching. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2017;180:88-97. [PubMed ID: 28923518]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.08.013.

  • 35.

    Hopkins ME, Nitecki R, Bucci DJ. Physical exercise during adolescence versus adulthood: differential effects on object recognition memory and brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels. Neuroscience. 2011;194:84-94. [PubMed ID: 21839807]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3183160]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.07.071.

  • 36.

    Kim H, Heo HI, Kim DH, Ko IG, Lee SS, Kim SE, et al. Treadmill exercise and methylphenidate ameliorate symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder through enhancing dopamine synthesis and brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression in spontaneous hypertensive rats. Neurosci Lett. 2011;504(1):35-9. [PubMed ID: 21907264]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.08.052.