The Effects of Sleep Inertia on the Acoustic and Perceptual Voice Changes in Adult University Students

authors:

avatar Mohammad-Amin Nazari 1 , avatar Kowsar Baghban ORCID 1 , avatar Mohammad-Sadegh Seifpanahi ORCID 2 , * , avatar Leili Tapak ORCID 3

Department of Speech and Language Pathology, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran
Department of Speech and Language Pathology, Autism Spectrum Disorders Research Center, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran
Department of Biostatics, School of Public Health, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran

how to cite: Nazari M, Baghban K, Seifpanahi M, Tapak L. The Effects of Sleep Inertia on the Acoustic and Perceptual Voice Changes in Adult University Students. Middle East J Rehabil Health Stud. 2024;11(4):e147131. https://doi.org/10.5812/mejrh-147131.

Abstract

Background:

Sleep inertia, characterized by reduced alertness and cognitive functioning upon waking, has been well-researched in its effects on cognitive and motor abilities. However, its impact on voice quality remains less clear.

Objectives:

This study aimed to investigate the relationship between sleep inertia following nocturnal sleep and changes in acoustic and perceptual voice parameters in adults.

Methods:

Ninety healthy participants (45 females, 45 males, ages: 18 - 30 years) took part in a pre-post study using convenience sampling. Their voices were recorded before and after nocturnal sleep across multiple tasks, including vowel /a/ phonation, spontaneous speech, text reading, and maximum phonation time (MPT). Recordings were made in a quiet room using a Zoom H6 recorder at a 44.1 kHz rate. The acoustic analysis included measures of fundamental frequency (F0), standard deviation of F0 (SD F0), vocal intensity, harmonics-to-noise ratio (HNR), cepstral peak prominence smoothed (CPPS), jitter, shimmer, and formant frequencies. Auditory-perceptual evaluation was conducted using the GRBAS Scale. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS-19. The Shapiro-Wilk test assessed normality, and a paired t-test compared variables, separately for males and females at a 0.05 significance level.

Results:

Acoustic analysis revealed that both females and males experienced a decrease in mean F0, mean intensity, HNR, and CPPS, and an increase in jitter, shimmer, and SD F0 across most voice samples after sleeping. These changes were particularly significant for females in all voice samples (P < 0.001). Perceptual analysis also showed a significant increase in grade, roughness, breathiness, asthenia, and strain for both genders after sleep (P < 0.001).

Conclusions:

This study demonstrated that sleep inertia negatively impacts vocal performance, resulting in a decline in voice quality for both genders after sleep compared to before. Notably, this decline in voice quality was more pronounced in women than in men.

1. Background

The term "sleep inertia" denotes a transient phase of drowsiness, confusion, and reduced cognitive abilities experienced immediately after waking up, as opposed to the state before falling asleep (1). Research highlights that sleep inertia affects a range of functions, including motor skills, cognitive performance (2), and alertness, with complex tasks being more vulnerable than simpler ones (3, 4). While vocal function is essential for communication and self-confidence (5), few studies have examined how sleep inertia impacts voice quality. Legros et al. (6) observed significant changes in fundamental frequency following short naps, and further research confirmed that sleep inertia also affects vowel sounds (7). Cavallero and Versace (8) reported that sleep deprivation exacerbates the effects of sleep inertia. Vocal fatigue, a result of sleep inertia, can lead to significant changes in voice quality (9, 10). Sleep inertia appears to be closely tied to the duration of slow-wave sleep, being more pronounced after longer naps with increased slow-wave activity. Additionally, voice disorders are influenced by sleep quality and stress (11, 12). While its direct impact on voice production requires further investigation, sleep inertia may affect muscle control, breathing, and resonance. Additionally, natural physiological processes during nocturnal sleep lead to a deeper, hoarser voice (13) upon waking, potentially impairing vocal performance for up to 30 minutes (14, 15).

The impact of sleep inertia on voice quality remains unclear, with mixed findings primarily focused on the effects following short-term naps during the day rather than after a full night's sleep (6, 7, 16). Icht et al. (17) recorded voice samples both after a night's sleep and after 24 hours of sleep deprivation, but comparing the voice before and immediately after sleep is essential due to the nature of sleep inertia. Previous research (6, 17) has mainly examined the acoustic qualities of voice. Despite advanced tools for acoustic, aerodynamic, and vocal imaging, auditory-perceptual evaluation is still the preferred method among voice therapists (18, 19). Our study focuses on key acoustic voice parameters such as jitter and shimmer, which assess the stability and smoothness of voice, with higher values often indicating roughness or breathiness and suggesting potential vocal issues. Monitoring these parameters can aid in assessing and enhancing overall voice quality (20). Another critical parameter, the harmonics-to-noise ratio (HNR), quantifies the level of additive noise in the voice signal (21). Additionally, smoothed cepstral peak prominence (CPPS) is recognized as a reliable metric for assessing dysphonic voice qualities in both single vowel and connected speech tasks (22, 23). Based on previous studies, a series of vocal tasks were selected for analysis: Sustained phonation, spontaneous speech, standard text reading, and maximum phonation time (MPT). These tasks offer a good variety and are ideal for analysis. Sustained phonation analyzes steady-state vocal qualities (24), spontaneous speech captures natural variations (25), standard text reading ensures uniformity, and MPT measures vocal fold endurance (26). This diverse selection aims to provide a comprehensive and accurate analysis of voice function. Recordings were made in a quiet room using a Zoom H6 recorder at a 44.1 kHz rate.

This study aimed to explore the impact of sleep inertia on acoustic and perceptual voice parameters in young adults by recording voices before and immediately after night sleep in both genders and comparing the outcomes. Unlike prior studies, this research recorded voices both before and after sleep to evaluate a wider range of acoustic parameters. It utilized the GRBAS Scale, recommended by Icht et al. (17) for comprehensive perceptual voice assessment, covering Grade, Roughness, Breathiness, Asthenia, and Strain from 0 (normal) to 3 (severe). Two experienced therapists conducted blind assessments using earphones, reviewing all samples except MPT. The study sought to understand how sleep inertia affected morning voice clarity, which is crucial in professions requiring optimal vocal performance upon waking. The research also examined how voice quality varied post-sleep between males and females, providing insights into the specific effects of sleep inertia on voice characteristics.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The study was confirmed by the Ethical Committee of Hamadan University of Medical Sciences ( IR.UMSHA.REC.1401.974) and comprised 90 healthy university students (native Persian speakers) with no reported voice issues affecting pitch, volume, tone, or overall quality. This group included 45 females (average age: 21.86 years, range: 19 - 24) and 45 males (average age: 23.75 years, range: 18 - 30). Participants were selected via convenience sampling, meeting inclusion criteria. The criteria for inclusion encompassed no history of head and neck surgery, absence of current voice disorders, normal hearing, no ongoing cold symptoms, and no voice changes related to smoking. Additionally, the demographic form recorded participants' consumption of liquids such as water and caffeinated drinks. To prevent potential confounding effects, participants were instructed not to consume any liquids after waking up during the second recording session.

2.2. Procedure

During data collection, two trained speech-language pathology students recorded participants' voices and provided thorough briefings on the study's objectives and protocols. Each participant's voice was recorded under two specific conditions, timed and sequenced to strategically analyze the effects of sleep on vocal characteristics. The sequence of recordings was structured as follows:

Initial recording: Conducted at night just before the participant went to sleep, this session aimed to document the baseline vocal condition following a typical day, unaffected by sleep.

Second recording: Scheduled for the morning immediately after the participant awoke, this session was designed to assess any changes in the voice that might have occurred due to sleep, comparing it with the baseline from the previous night.

Participants were told to return the next morning for a second recording after the initial night session. They self-reported their night's sleep duration along with their age and Body Mass Index (BMI) in the demographic questionnaire. Body Mass Index is calculated by dividing weight in kilograms by height in meters squared (kg/m2) and classifies individuals as underweight, normal, overweight, or obese. Participants' sleep quality or sleep disorders were documented in their demographic information using the "Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index" (PSQI) Questionnaire. The PSQI is a 19-item self-report questionnaire used to assess sleep quality. It is one of the most widely used and important tools for evaluating sleep quality.

2.3. Voice Sampling

Previous research has shown that sustained vowels and connected speech are ideal for recording voice samples (24). One of the voice recordings used in the study was a standard text reading task ensuring uniform speech samples across individuals, minimizing the influence of speech speed and patterns. Despite this, spontaneous speech was included because it effectively highlights differences between normal and abnormal voice production (25). Maximum Phonation Time, a common clinical measure, assesses the phonation mechanism by determining the longest duration a vowel like /a/ can be sustained (26). All of the voice recordings employed for acoustic analysis included sustained phonation, spontaneous speech, standard text reading, and MPT, as illustrated in Figure 1. Given the variations in voice acoustic parameters noted in earlier studies for different speech tasks (27), our study incorporates diverse voice samples for comprehensive acoustic analysis.

Illustrative voice recordings employed for acoustic analysis: A, sustained phonation task; B, spontaneous speech task; C, "standard text" reading task; and D, maximum phonation time (MPT) task.
Illustrative voice recordings employed for acoustic analysis: A, sustained phonation task; B, spontaneous speech task; C,

During each experimental session, participants were required to produce four types of voice samples in both recording sessions:

(1) Sustained phonation: Participants sustained the vowel /a/ for five seconds at a comfortable loudness and constant pitch.

(2) Spontaneous speech: Participants introduced themselves by stating their first and last names, field of study, and academic semester.

(3) Standard text reading: Participants read the Persian version of "The Grandfather Passage" (25).

(4) Maximum phonation time: Participants took a deep breath and then sustained the vowel /a/ for as long as possible, repeating this three times.

All voice samples were recorded in a quiet room in the student dormitory of Hamadan University of Medical Sciences. A Zoom H6 handy recorder (Zoom Corporation, Japan) was used, set to a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz. The Zoom H6 is a portable recorder featuring interchangeable mic capsules, six-track simultaneous recording up to 24-bit/96kHz, and four XLR/TRS inputs. It doubles as a multi-channel audio interface, making it ideal for versatile audio recording needs. The recorder was placed horizontally, slightly above the participant's lips at an equal distance of 10 cm from their mouth to record their voices.

2.4. Acoustic Analysis and Maximum Phonation Time

Voice samples were analyzed using Praat software version 6.2.03 (28). For sustained phonation tasks, analysis focused on a 4-second middle segment, excluding the initial and final half-seconds (29). All voice samples, except for MPT, were assessed for fundamental frequency (F0) in Hz, standard deviation of F0 (SD F0) in Hz, voice intensity in dB, HNR in dB, jitter, shimmer in percentage, and cepstral peak prominence-smoothed (CPPS) in dB. Additionally, the first two formant frequencies (F1, F2) of the vowel /a/ were also measured. Maximum phonation time was calculated as the average of three attempts per participant. The CPPS calculation followed the method described by Watts et al. (30). Each voice sample type (sustained phonation /a/, spontaneous speech, grandfather passage reading, and MPT) was analyzed separately for each participant in a blind manner to avoid bias.

2.5. Auditory-Perceptual Evaluation

The GRBAS Scale was used for auditory-perceptual evaluations of voice samples, assessing Grade (G), Roughness (R), Breathiness (B), Asthenia (A), and Strain (S) on a scale from zero (normal) to three (severe problem). Two experienced voice therapists with over four years of practice performed blind assessments in a quiet room using bilateral earphones, reviewing all samples except for MPT. To ensure unbiased evaluations, voice samples were randomly coded, keeping raters unaware of the subjects' identities and the recording times. Inter-rater reliability was measured by having the therapists independently score the samples twice, one week apart. The GRBAS Scale was used for conducting auditory-perceptual evaluations of the recorded voice samples. The inter-rater reliability for the GRBAS parameters was measured using the weighted kappa coefficient, showing strong agreement: Grade (0.751), roughness (0.807), breathiness (0.839), asthenia (0.779), and strain (0.760), all considered excellent as values above 0.75. Intra-rater reliability was also assessed by having therapists reevaluate 15% of the samples, resulting in consistent kappa values: Grade (0.845), roughness (0.846), breathiness (0.876), asthenia (0.828), and strain (0.714), indicating high agreement for most parameters.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS-19 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois). A Shapiro-Wilk test was used to assess the normality of the differences between two dependent variables in both acoustic and auditory-perceptual analyses. A paired t-test was then conducted to compare these variables. Due to gender differences in voice characteristics and parameters, all analyses were performed separately for male and female participants at a significance level of 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic Characteristics

Demographic characteristics, including age, BMI, and sleep duration (in minutes), are depicted in Table 1. Applying the independent samples t-test showed a significant difference in sleep duration between the two gender groups (P = 0.015). Females reported an average sleep duration of 43.2 minutes longer than males.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics According to the Gender a

VariablesAgeBMISleep Duration
Sex
Female21.86 (1.23)22.40 (3.30)406.8 (79.2)
Male23.75 (2.89)23.73 (3.55)363.6 (112.8)
P-value b< 0.0010.804< 0.05

3.2. Acoustic Analysis of Voice Quality, Maximum Phonation Time, and Auditory-Perceptual Evaluation

The mean and standard deviation for various acoustic voice parameters across three voice samples (sustained phonation /a/, spontaneous speech, and grandfather passage), measured before and after sleep in both male and female participants, along with the effect size values and their related rankings (low, medium, and high effect size) for each acoustic parameter, are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Mean ± SD Values of Acoustic Parameters Measured Before and Immediately After Nocturnal Sleep Using Paired t-Test, and the Effect Sizes Across All Types of Voice Samples for Female (a) and Male (b) Participants a

Type of Voice Sample and Acoustic ParameterBefore Nocturnal SleepAfter Nocturnal SleepDifference (After-Before)Effect Size (Rankings: H, M, L)95% CI for DifferenceP- Value b
(a) Female participants, Sustained phonation /a/
Mean F0 (Hz)215.40 (21.12)197.36 (35.69)- 18.04 (29.74)- 0.60 (M) (- 27.29, - 8.79)< 0.001
SD1 F0 (Hz)5.25 (11.29)19.30 (19.30)14.05 (21.81)0.64 (M) (6.43, 21.67)< 0.001
Mean intensity (dB)58.94 (4.49)52.90 (3.33)- 6.04 (4.67)- 1.29 (H) (- 7.12, - 4.96)< 0.001
HNR (dB)21.86 (3.31)17.57 (3.85)- 4.28 (3.44)- 1.24 (H) (- 5.09, - 3.47)< 0.001
CPPS (dB)13.13 (1.40)10.28 (1.91)- 2.85 (2.28)- 1.25 (H) (- 3.37, - 2.33)< 0.001
Jitter 0.39 (0.24)0.80 (0.50)0.31 (0.45)0.68 (M) (0.19, 0.43)< 0.001
Shimmer 3.25 (1.24)5.43 (2.19)1.34 (2.13)0.62 (M) (0.77, 1.91)< 0.001
Mean F1 (Hz) 691.45 (83.52)553.68 (112.70)- 137.76 (93.11)- 1.47 (H) (- 171.92, - 103.60)< 0.001
Mean F2 (Hz) 1202.78 (86.89)1159.18 (79.44)- 43.59 (103.15)- 0.42 (M) (- 75.52, - 11.66)< 0.001
Spontaneous speech
Mean F0 (Hz)220.87 (21.40)203.10 (31.54)- 17.77 (27.88)- 0.63 (M) (- 27.74, - 7.80)< 0.001
SD1 F0 (Hz)44.92 (23.08)52.27 (24.75)7.35 (21.51)0.34 (L) (0.50, 14.20)< 0.05
Mean intensity (dB)56.20 (3.23)51.02 (2.96)- 5.17 (2.85)- 1.81 (H) (- 6.33, - 4.01)< 0.001
HNR (dB)16.19 (1.52)14.90 (2.19)- 1.28 (2.21)- 0.57 (M) (- 2.14, - 0.42)< 0.001
CPPS (dB)8.95 (1.23)7.05 (1.22)- 1.90 (1.450)- 1.31 (H) (- 2.33, - 1.47)< 0.001
Jitter 1.55 (0.30)1.87 (0.50)0.31 (0.45)0.68 (M) (0.14, 0.48)< 0.001
Shimmer 6.71 (1.01)8.05 (2.07)1.34 (2.13)0.62 (M) (0.52, 2.16)< 0.001
Grandfather passage
Mean F0 (Hz)210.82 (18.31)194.95 (23.57)- 15.87 (20.82)- 0.76 (H) (- 23.32, - 8.42)< 0.001
SD1 F0 (Hz)45.15 (10.88)57.55 (14.00)12.39 (13.06)0.94 (H) (7.82, 16.96)< 0.001
Mean intensity (dB)57.26 (3.26)52.36 (3.05)- 4.89 (2.90)1.68 (H) (- 5.90, - 3.88)< 0.001
HNR (dB)15.60 (1.23)14.54 (1.96)- 1.06 (1.57)- 0.67 (M) (- 1.59, - 0.53)< 0.001
CPPS (dB)9.02 (0.94)7.24 (1.15)- 1.78 (1.16)- 1.53 (H) (- 2.24, - 1.32)< 0.001
Jitter 1.66 (0.27)2.04 (0.50)0.37 (0.42)0.88 (H) (0.20, 0.54)< 0.001
Shimmer 7.25 (0.73)8.32 (1.67)1.07 (1.60)0.66 (M) (0.51, 1.63)< 0.001
Maximum phonation time (MPT)
MPT (s)13.87 (4.28)11.40 (3.48)- 2.47 (2.95)- 0.83 (H) (- 3.53, - 1.41)< 0.001
(b) Male participants sustained phonation /a/
Mean F0 (Hz)117.24 (14.29)106.56 (12.00)- 10.67 (9.53)- 1.11 (H) (- 14.08, - 7.26)< 0.001
SD1 F0 (Hz)1.28 (0.67)1.76 (1.39)0.48 (1.41)0.34 (L) (- 0.03, 0.99)< 0.001
Mean intensity (dB)61.83 (3.50)59.20 (4.27)- 2.62 (3.87)- 0.67 (M) (- 4.01, - 1.23)< 0.001
HNR (dB)21.31 (3.82)19.43 (3.95)- 1.87 (3.23)- 0.57 (M) (- 3.02, - 0.72)< 0.001
CPPS (dB)15.87 (2.48)14.18 (2.83)- 1.69 (2.50)- 0.67 (M) (- 2.59, - 0.79)< 0.001
Jitter 0.42 (0.25)0.93 (1.40)0.50 (1.41)0.35 (L) (- 0.01, 1.01)< 0.05
Shimmer 3.54 (1.80)4.24 (2.65)0.69 (2.36)0.29 (L) (- 0.15, 1.53)0.054
Mean F1 (Hz)566.76 (47.71)530.80 (63.46)- 35.95 (41.44)- 0.86 (H) (- 50.78, - 21.12)< 0.001
Mean F2 (Hz)1031.18 (71.54)1019.62 (72.59)- 11.55 (75.26)- 0.15 (L) (- 38.47, 15.37)0.309
Spontaneous speech
Mean F0 (Hz)125.74 (15.04)115.72 (13.35)- 10.02 (12.58)- 0.79 (H) (- 14.53, - 5.51)< 0.001
SD1 F0 (Hz)33.53 (28.27)33.97 (35.68)0.43 (41.20)0.01 (L) (- 14.31, 15.17)0.943
Mean intensity (dB)57.50 (2.28)55.80 (2.66)- 1.69 (2.69)- 0.62 (M) (- 2.65, - 0.73)< 0.001
HNR (dB)12.97 (2.26)12.62 (2.14)- 0.35 (1.78)- 0.19 (L) (- 1.00, 0.30)0.190
CPPS (dB)8.74 (1.36)8.06 (1.32)- 0.68 (1.23)- 0.55 (M) (- 1.11, - 0.25)< 0.001
Jitter 2.16 (0.56)2.65 (1.07)0.48 (1.16)0.41 (M) (0.07, 0.89)< 0.001
Shimmer 9.79 (2.39)10.31 (2.17)0.51 (2.18)0.23 (L) (- 0.28, 1.30)0.119
Grandfather passage
Mean F0 (Hz)125.02 (15.16)113.63 (12.60)- 11.38 (9.61)- 1.18 (H) (- 14.81, - 7.95)< 0.001
SD1 F0 (Hz)31.28 (15.34)35.78 (22.32)4.50 (16.15)0.27 (L) (- 1.28, 10.28)< 0.001
Mean intensity (dB)59.61 (2.76)56.78 (2.59)- 2.83 (2.31)- 1.22 (H) (- 3.65, - 2.01)< 0.001
HNR (dB)11.77 (1.83)11.87 (1.99)0.10 (1.27)0.07 (L) (- 0.35, 0.55)0.597
CPPS (dB)8.58 (1.04)8.20 (1.18)- 0.37 (1.00)- 0.37 (L) (- 0.72, - 0.02)< 0.05
Jitter 2.26 (0.35)2.63 (0.67)0.37 (0.58)0.63 (M) (0.15, 0.59)< 0.001
Shimmer 10.53 (1.76)10.70 (2.04)0.17 (1.66)0.10 (L) (- 0.42, 0.76)0.488
Maximum phonation time
MPT (s)20.19 (6.55)18.28 (6.41)- 1.91 (5.56)- 0.34 (L) (- 3.89, - 0.93)< 0.05

Significant findings include a decrease in mean F0, mean intensity, HNR, and CPPS for all voice samples in females, and specifically F1 and F2 during sustained phonation /a/. Additionally, increases in the SD F0, jitter, and shimmer were observed across all samples. In males, there were notable decreases in mean F0, mean intensity, and CPPS across all voice samples, while jitter showed a significant increase across all samples. Both genders also experienced a significant reduction in MPT. The effect size results showed that in the female group, all acoustic parameters exhibited medium to high effect sizes, except for the SD F0 in spontaneous speech, which was low. Conversely, in the male group, the effect sizes for the studied acoustic parameters ranged from low to high.

Table 2 provides detailed statistics and effect sizes for each parameter, reflecting the impact of sleep inertia on vocal characteristics and highlighting gender differences in these effects.

3.3. Acoustic Analysis of Voice Quality, Maximum Phonation Time, and Auditory-Perceptual Evaluation

- The mean and standard deviation for various acoustic voice parameters across three voice samples (sustained phonation /a/, spontaneous speech, and grandfather passage), measured before and after sleep in both male and female participants, along with the effect size values and their related rankings (low, medium, and high effect size) for each acoustic parameter, are presented in Table 2.

- Significant findings include a decrease in mean F0, mean intensity, HNR, and CPPS for all voice samples in females, and specifically F1 and F2 during sustained phonation /a/. Additionally, increases in the SD F0, jitter, and shimmer were observed across all samples. In males, there were notable decreases in mean F0, mean intensity, and CPPS across all voice samples, while jitter showed a significant increase across all samples. Both genders also experienced a significant reduction in MPT. The effect size results showed that in the female group, all acoustic parameters exhibited medium to high effect sizes, except for the SD F0 in spontaneous speech, which was low. Conversely, in the male group, the effect sizes for the studied acoustic parameters ranged from low to high.

Table 2 provides detailed statistics and effect sizes for each parameter, reflecting the impact of sleep inertia on vocal characteristics and highlighting gender differences in these effects.

3.4. Auditory-Perceptual Analysis

- The results of auditory-perceptual analysis by gender, (a) for females and (b) for males, are shown in Table 3. For a better review of the results, the GRBAS test findings in female and male groups are presented in Figure 2. The difference in the total GRBAS score was 3.84 (P < 0.001) for females and 3.58 (P < 0.001) for males, indicating slightly greater auditory-perceptual changes in females compared to males. The effect size for all GRBAS subscales was high for both genders, with the exception of strain, which exhibited a medium effect size.

Changes in mean values of GRBAS auditory-perceptual parameters before and after night sleep in A, female; and B, male participants
Changes in mean values of GRBAS auditory-perceptual parameters before and after night sleep in A, female; and B, male participants

Figure 2 shows the changes in mean values of GRBAS auditory-perceptual parameters before and after night sleep, illustrating the overall impact of sleep inertia on voice quality for both genders.

Table 3.

Results of GRBAS Scores Before and Immediately After Nocturnal Sleep Rated Through Perceptual Analysis for Female (a) and Male (b) Participants a

GRBAS Scale ItemsBefore Nocturnal SleepAfter Nocturnal SleepDifference (After-Before)Effect Size (Rankings: H, M, L)95% CI for DifferenceP-Value b
(a) Females
Grade0.00 (0.00)0.95 (0.79)0.95 (0.79)1.20 (H)(0.75, 1.15)< 0.001
Roughness 0.02 (0.14)0.95 (0.52)0.93 (0.53)1.75 (H)(0.78, 1.08)< 0.001
Breathiness 0.00 (0.00)0.66 (0.63)0.66 (0.63)1.04 (H)(0.49, 0.83)< 0.001
Asthenia 0.08 (0.28)1.11 (0.53)1.02 (0.62)1.64 (H)(0.86, 1.18)< 0.001
Strain0.00 (0.00)0.26 (0.44)0.26 (0.44)0.59 (M)(0.14, 0.38)< 0.001
Total score0.11 (0.38)3.95 (2.23)3.84 (2.26)1.69 (H)(3.28, 4.40)< 0.001
(b) Males
Grade0.00 (0.00)0.97 (0.75)0.97 (0.75)1.29 (H)(0.77, 1.17)< 0.001
Roughness 0.04 (0.20)1.00 (0.52)0.96 (0.52)1.84 (H)(0.82, 1.10)< 0.001
Breathiness 0.02 (0.14)0.57 (0.54)0.55 (0.54)1.01 (H)(0.42, 0.68)< 0.001
Asthenia 0.04 (0.20)1.04 (0.42)1.00 (0.47)2.12 (H)(0.86, 1.14)< 0.001
Strain0.00 (0.00)0.20 (0.40)0.20 (0.40)0.5 (M)(0.10, 0.30)< 0.001
Total score0.11 (0.31)3.80 (2.00)3.68 (1.92)1.91 (H)(3.12, 4.24)< 0.001

4. Discussion

"Sleep inertia" refers to drowsiness and reduced cognitive function upon waking. While its effects on motor skills and cognition are well-studied (6, 17, 31), its impact on voice quality remains less explored. This study fills this gap by analyzing voice changes before and after sleep using acoustic and auditory perceptual analyses.

This study noted significant reductions in mean F0 and mean intensity in both genders after night sleep, consistent with findings by Icht et al. (17), who studied 24-hour sleep deprivation. Contrarily, Legros et al. (6) reported increases in mean F0 after short naps, a discrepancy likely explained by different sleep durations between the studies.

Both males and females showed decreases in HNR and CPPS values post-sleep, in line with Icht et al. (17), who also observed a decrease in HNR after night sleep, with the reduction being more pronounced in women compared to men. The effect size of the reduction in HNR and CPPS in the female group compared to men ranged from medium to high, while it ranged from low to medium across different speech tasks. This suggests a noisier and more irregular voice after night sleep in women. Previous studies have not investigated the Effect Size of the differences in acoustic parameters after night sleep, making it impossible to compare this finding with past studies. Lower HNR values often seen in pathological voices (32). The CPP quantifies energy in the main harmonic, reflecting voice organization, while CPPS enhances this by averaging across quefrency and time domains for improved clarity (33, 34). Lower CPPS values are typically linked to pathological conditions, indicating more abnormal voices (33, 35). Our findings of reduced CPPS align with those of Hasanvand et al. (35), who observed similar trends in individuals with voice disorders.

Our study observed increased jitter and shimmer values for both sexes after sleep, consistent with previous findings (17). Standard deviation F0 values also rose significantly across most vocal tasks, except in spontaneous speech. Jitter, shimmer, and SD F0, which measure short-term frequency and amplitude variations, are crucial for detecting voice abnormalities and assessing the severity of voice deviations (36-38). Higher values of jitter and shimmer, along with variable SD F0, indicate disturbances in voice quality, suggesting that sleep affects voice consistency and quality (39, 40).

After night sleep, both men and women showed a significant decrease in MPT, an essential tool for assessing voice disorders and treatment efficacy (41). Decreases in MPT can indicate issues like insufficient inspiratory volume or glottal resistance, leading to breathiness (42). Consistent with earlier studies (43, 44), MPT is lower for patients with voice disorders than normal subjects. In this study, pre-sleep MPT values were normal—13.87 seconds for women and 20.19 for men—but post-sleep, MPT decreased for both genders, particularly falling below normal for women (45). This reduction is likely due to overnight relaxation of respiratory muscles, weakening muscle strength and affecting respiratory support, as described by Schwarz et al. (46). These results underscore the importance of further research into post-sleep changes in MPT and respiratory functions, advocating for more detailed aerodynamic assessments.

Perceptual analysis with the GRBAS scale revealed significant increases in all perceptual parameters for both genders, suggesting that voice quality becomes harsher, rougher, breathier, weaker, and more strained after sleep due to the effects of sleep inertia. The effect sizes for differences in all GRBAS subscales for both genders after night sleep were high, except for "strain," which was medium. This may be attributed to the inherent characteristics of the "strain" subscale.

The statistical significance of the reported changes in acoustic parameters and GRBAS scores reveals important insights into the impact of sleep inertia on voice quality in both male and female participants. Females showed significant decreases in F0, intensity, HNR, and CPPS across all voice samples, with increased SD F0, jitter, and shimmer. Males experienced reductions in F0, intensity, CPPS, HNR, and F1 during sustained phonation /a/, along with increased SD F0 and jitter. Both genders also saw reduced MPT, indicating compromised vocal endurance. Effect size analysis revealed medium to high effects in females for most parameters, whereas males exhibited varied effects. Auditory-perceptual analysis using the GRBAS scale showed significantly higher total score changes in females (3.84, P < 0.001) compared to males (3.58, P < 0.001), with high effect sizes across most GRBAS subscales except for strain, which had a medium effect size. The more pronounced acoustic and perceptual changes in women's voices compared to men's can be attributed to gender-related anatomical and physiological differences in the vocal folds (47). Female vocal folds are approximately 20 - 30% thinner and contain less collagen, elastin, and hyaluronic acid (48, 49), making them more susceptible to injury and more sensitive to physiological changes during sleep that affect voice quality (50). Hormonal fluctuations, especially related to the menstrual cycle, further influence vocal fold function and voice quality in women. Additionally, effect size comparisons for most acoustic parameters and MPT revealed that women's voices are more affected by nocturnal sleep than men's. Consequently, all acoustic parameters, including fundamental frequency, jitter, shimmer, and harmonic-to-noise ratio (HNR), show significant changes upon waking in females, whereas in males, only certain parameters like shimmer and HNR are significantly affected. Maximum phonation time is also more impacted in women. These findings underscore the importance of tailored voice therapy, particularly for female patients, and highlight the need for awareness and management of sleep patterns and wake-up routines among professionals who rely heavily on their voices.

In general, the possible reasons may explain the observed changes in voice parameters summarized as follows:

Increased muscle relaxation and reduced vocal fold activity: During sleep, muscle relaxation leads to partial larynx closure, disrupting vocal fold vibrations and reducing voice quality, resulting in lower mean F0, mean intensity, and MPT, and higher jitter, shimmer, and SD F0 (31). Relaxation also shifts the positions of the tongue, palate, and vocal tract, affecting formant frequencies (51). Tongue positioning significantly influences formant frequencies, crucial for determining vowel sounds. F1 is inversely correlated with tongue height (higher F1, lower tongue), and F2 is associated with tongue backness and lip rounding (higher F2, more front tongue, less rounded lips) (52). Upon waking, reduced muscle tension alters vocal tract resonances, changing mean F1 and F2, suggesting a more elevated and posterior tongue position during sleep (53). Future studies should consider using advanced imaging technologies like MRI or ultrasound to directly examine tongue positioning during sleep and its effects on vocal tract configurations and voice production.

Decreased throat lubrication and vocal fold hydration: Sleep reduces saliva production and swallowing, leading to fluid buildup and mucus on the vocal folds, which can cause slight swelling (27). This swelling disrupts vocal fold vibrations, resulting in a rougher voice after sleep (13). Additionally, mouth breathing during sleep dehydrates the airway and dries vocal fold membranes (54), potentially causing vocal strain (13, 55). These conditions can lead to decreased CPPS and HNR values, and increased jitter, shimmer, and SD F0 values in acoustic assessments, along with elevated perceptual parameters post-sleep.

Nocturnal acid reflux: Nocturnal acid reflux is associated with the development of laryngopharyngeal reflux disease (LPRD), which is frequently observed in individuals with vocal disorders. In fact, LPRD is diagnosed in over 50% of patients who present with voice-related complaints (56). Acid reflux can also lead to a hoarse voice in the morning (57). Patients with laryngopharyngeal reflux usually complain of voice problems such as burning in the throat area, throat clearing, chronic cough, hoarseness, or sore throat, especially in the morning (15, 58).

4.1. Limitations

The current study has some limitations. Firstly, it targets a young demographic (18 - 30 years) and may not represent voice changes across all age groups, limiting its generalizability. Future research should include a broader age range, including older and younger individuals, in order to enhance the analysis of voice quality. Secondly, the study did not consider confounding factors such as diet, which could influence the outcomes. Future studies should explore these variables to better understand sleep's impact on voice quality. Lastly, the study lacked a third measurement to assess voice normalization post-sleep disturbance. Including a follow-up measurement in future studies is recommended to monitor the return to normal voice states. Furthermore, in addition to the previously mentioned points, utilizing advanced technologies like ultrasound imaging of the tongue can provide a more precise assessment of tongue movements and positioning during sleep. Additionally, future research should explore the intricate connection between various sleep stages and their impact on voice production.

4.2. Conclusions

This study demonstrates that sleep inertia, characterized by reduced functionality and alertness upon waking, negatively impacts voice performance in university students aged 18 to 30, leading to decreased voice quality in both genders. By focusing on this specific age group, the research highlights the significant effects of sleep inertia on young adult voice quality, emphasizing the need for further investigation in this area. Notably, the study's strengths include its inclusion of both genders, a substantial sample size, the use of the GRBAS scale for perceptual evaluation, and a comprehensive analysis of sleep inertia.

These findings enhance our understanding of the relationship between sleep and voice health, which is crucial for improving clinical assessments and treatments of voice disorders in young adults. Voice therapists should consider sleep patterns and inertia when planning sessions, especially for female students who may experience greater voice deterioration upon waking. Scheduling therapy sessions when patients are least affected by sleep inertia can optimize vocal performance and outcomes. Promoting good sleep hygiene practices, such as maintaining regular sleep schedules and creating a restful environment, is vital to reduce the effects of sleep inertia.

Professionals in this age group who rely heavily on their voices, such as university students, singers, teachers, and call center employees, should be informed about the impact of sleep inertia. Employers might consider these effects when arranging work shifts, particularly for female staff who may need more time to overcome sleep inertia. Additionally, this research can aid in developing recovery strategies and techniques for voice rehabilitation, helping speech and language pathologists improve voice treatments for young adults.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Lubin A, Hord DJ, Tracy ML, Johnson LC. Effects of exercise, bedrest and napping on performance decrement during 40 hours. Psychophysiol. 1976;13(4):334-9. [PubMed ID: 951475]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1976.tb03086.x.

  • 2.

    Jewett ME, Wyatt JK, Ritz-De Cecco A, Khalsa SB, Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Time course of sleep inertia dissipation in human performance and alertness. J Sleep Res. 1999;8(1):1-8. [PubMed ID: 10188130]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.1999.00128.x.

  • 3.

    Burke TM, Scheer F, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP. Sleep inertia, sleep homeostatic and circadian influences on higher-order cognitive functions. J Sleep Res. 2015;24(4):364-71. [PubMed ID: 25773686]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5124508]. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12291.

  • 4.

    Muzet A, Nicolas A, Tassi P, Dewasmes G, Bonneau A. Implementation of napping in industry and the problem of sleep inertia. J Sleep Res. 1995;4(S2):67-9. [PubMed ID: 10607215]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00230.x.

  • 5.

    Ryu CH, Han S, Lee MS, Kim SY, Nam SY, Roh JL, et al. Voice Changes in Elderly Adults: Prevalence and the Effect of Social, Behavioral, and Health Status on Voice Quality. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63(8):1608-14. [PubMed ID: 26140657]. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.13559.

  • 6.

    Legros C, Ruiz R, Plantin De Hugues P. Analysing cockpit and laboratory recordings to determine fatigue levels in pilots' voices. J Acoustical Soc Am. 2008;123(5_Supplement):3070. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2932837.

  • 7.

    Ruiz R, Plantin de Hugues P, Legros C, editors. Amplitude modulation of vowel glottal pulses: application to sleep inertia. Acoustics 2012. 2012; Nantes, France. Hal Open Science; https://hal.science/hal-00810601/document https://hal.science/hal-00810601/file/hal-00810601.pdf.

  • 8.

    Cavallero C, Versace F. Stage at Awakening, Sleep Inertia and Performance. Sleep Res Online: SRO. 2003;5:89-97.

  • 9.

    Nwe TL, Li H, Dong M. Analysis and detection of speech under sleep deprivation.In: Nwe TL, Li H, Dong M, editors. INTERSPEECH. 2006. p. paper 1934-Wed2BuP.15-0.

  • 10.

    Raghavan S. Applications of large vocabulary continuous speech recognition to fatigue detection [thesis]. Mississippi State: Mississippi State University; 2006.

  • 11.

    Giddens CL, Barron KW, Byrd-Craven J, Clark KF, Winter AS. Vocal indices of stress: a review. J Voice. 2013;27(3):390 e21-9. [PubMed ID: 23462686]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2012.12.010.

  • 12.

    Cho JH, Guilminault C, Joo YH, Jin SK, Han KD, Park CS. A possible association between dysphonia and sleep duration: A cross-sectional study based on the Korean National Health and nutrition examination surveys from 2010 to 2012. PLoS One. 2017;12(8). e0182286. [PubMed ID: 28783741]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5544220]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182286.

  • 13.

    Berkley S. Speak to influence: How to unlock the hidden power of your voice. Chicago, IL: Campbell Hall Press; 2004.

  • 14.

    Baldwin DC, Daugherty SR. Sleep deprivation and fatigue in residency training: results of a national survey of first- and second-year residents. Sleep. 2004;27(2):217-23. [PubMed ID: 15124713]. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/27.2.217.

  • 15.

    Sataloff RT, Hawkshaw MJ, Gupta R. Laryngopharyngeal reflux and voice disorders: an overview on disease mechanisms, treatments, and research advances. Discov Med. 2010;10(52):213-24.

  • 16.

    Ruiz R, de Hugues PP, Legros C. Advanced Voice Analysis of Pilots to Detect Fatigue and Sleep Inertia. Acta Acust United Acust. 2010;96(3):567-79. https://doi.org/10.3813/aaa.918307.

  • 17.

    Icht M, Zukerman G, Hershkovich S, Laor T, Heled Y, Fink N, et al. The "Morning Voice": The Effect of 24 Hours of Sleep Deprivation on Vocal Parameters of Young Adults. J Voice. 2020;34(3):489 e1-9. [PubMed ID: 30527970]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2018.11.010.

  • 18.

    Behrman A. Common practices of voice therapists in the evaluation of patients. J Voice. 2005;19(3):454-69. [PubMed ID: 16102671]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2004.08.004.

  • 19.

    Webb AL, Carding PN, Deary IJ, MacKenzie K, Steen N, Wilson JA. The reliability of three perceptual evaluation scales for dysphonia. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2004;261(8):429-34. [PubMed ID: 14615893]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00405-003-0707-7.

  • 20.

    Behlau M. Voz: o livro do especialista. Rio de Janeiro, Brazil: Revinter Ltda; 2001.

  • 21.

    Ferrand CT. Harmonics-to-noise ratio: an index of vocal aging. J Voice. 2002;16(4):480-7. [PubMed ID: 12512635]. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-1997(02)00123-6.

  • 22.

    Maryn Y, Weenink D. Objective dysphonia measures in the program Praat: smoothed cepstral peak prominence and acoustic voice quality index. J Voice. 2015;29(1):35-43. [PubMed ID: 25499526]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2014.06.015.

  • 23.

    Patel RR, Awan SN, Barkmeier-Kraemer J, Courey M, Deliyski D, Eadie T, et al. Recommended Protocols for Instrumental Assessment of Voice: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Expert Panel to Develop a Protocol for Instrumental Assessment of Vocal Function. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2018;27(3):887-905. [PubMed ID: 29955816]. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-17-0009.

  • 24.

    Gerratt BR, Kreiman J, Garellek M. Comparing Measures of Voice Quality From Sustained Phonation and Continuous Speech. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016;59(5):994-1001. [PubMed ID: 27626612]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5345563]. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-15-0307.

  • 25.

    Roy N, Barkmeier-Kraemer J, Eadie T, Sivasankar MP, Mehta D, Paul D, et al. Evidence-based clinical voice assessment: a systematic review. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2013;22(2):212-26. [PubMed ID: 23184134]. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/12-0014).

  • 26.

    Maslan J, Leng X, Rees C, Blalock D, Butler SG. Maximum phonation time in healthy older adults. J Voice. 2011;25(6):709-13. [PubMed ID: 21439778]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3128209]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2010.10.002.

  • 27.

    Gates RK. The owner's manual to the singing voice. Columbus, United States: Ohio State University; 2003.

  • 28.

    Boersma P, Weenink D. Praat Manual: doing phonetics by computer. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Universiteit van Amsterdam; 2009, [cited 2023]. Available from: https://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/.

  • 29.

    Van Lieshout P. PRAAT -- Short Tutorial -- An introduction. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto; 2017, [cited 2023]. Available from: https://web.stanford.edu/dept/linguistics/corpora/material/PRAAT_workshop_manual_v421.pdf.

  • 30.

    Watts CR, Awan SN, Maryn Y. A Comparison of Cepstral Peak Prominence Measures From Two Acoustic Analysis Programs. J Voice. 2017;31(3):387 e1-387 e10. [PubMed ID: 27751661]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2016.09.012.

  • 31.

    Bagnall AD, Dorrian J, Fletcher A. Some vocal consequences of sleep deprivation and the possibility of "fatigue proofing" the voice with Voicecraft(R) voice training. J Voice. 2011;25(4):447-61. [PubMed ID: 21402470]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2010.10.020.

  • 32.

    Parsa V, Jamieson DG. Identification of pathological voices using glottal noise measures. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2000;43(2):469-85. [PubMed ID: 10757697]. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4302.469.

  • 33.

    Heman-Ackah YD, Michael DD, Goding GJ. The relationship between cepstral peak prominence and selected parameters of dysphonia. J Voice. 2002;16(1):20-7. [PubMed ID: 12008652]. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-1997(02)00067-x.

  • 34.

    Heman-Ackah YD. Reliability of calculating the cepstral peak without linear regression analysis. J Voice. 2004;18(2):203-8. [PubMed ID: 15193653]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2004.01.005.

  • 35.

    Hasanvand A, Salehi A, Ebrahimipour M. A Cepstral Analysis of Normal and Pathologic Voice Qualities in Iranian Adults: A Comparative Study. J Voice. 2017;31(4):508 e17-23. [PubMed ID: 27993499]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2016.10.017.

  • 36.

    Lee SH, Hong KH, Kim JS, Hong YT. Perceptual and Acoustic Outcomes of Early-Stage Glottic Cancer After Laser Surgery or Radiotherapy: A Meta-Analysis. Clin Exp Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;12(3):241-8. [PubMed ID: 30959580]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6635706]. https://doi.org/10.21053/ceo.2018.00990.

  • 37.

    Li G, Hou Q, Zhang C, Jiang Z, Gong S. Acoustic parameters for the evaluation of voice quality in patients with voice disorders. Ann Palliat Med. 2021;10(1):130-6. [PubMed ID: 33440977]. https://doi.org/10.21037/apm-20-2102.

  • 38.

    Lopes LW, Batista Simoes L, Delfino da Silva J, da Silva Evangelista D, da Nobrega EA, Oliveira Costa Silva P, et al. Accuracy of Acoustic Analysis Measurements in the Evaluation of Patients With Different Laryngeal Diagnoses. J Voice. 2017;31(3):382 e15-26. [PubMed ID: 27742492]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2016.08.015.

  • 39.

    Teixeira JP, Gonçalves A. Accuracy of Jitter and Shimmer Measurements. Procedia Technol. 2014;16:1190-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.protcy.2014.10.134.

  • 40.

    Teixeira JP, Fernandes PO. Acoustic Analysis of Vocal Dysphonia. Procedia Computer Sci. 2015;64:466-73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2015.08.544.

  • 41.

    Speyer R. Effects of voice therapy: a systematic review. J Voice. 2008;22(5):565-80. [PubMed ID: 17509828]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.10.005.

  • 42.

    Barsties B, De Bodt M. Assessment of voice quality: Current state-of-the-art. Auris Nasus Larynx. 2015;42(3):183-8. [PubMed ID: 25440411]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anl.2014.11.001.

  • 43.

    Raes JP, Clement PA. Aerodynamic measurements of voice production. Acta oto-rhino-laryngologica Belgica. 1996;50(4):293-8.

  • 44.

    Speyer R, Bogaardt HC, Passos VL, Roodenburg NP, Zumach A, Heijnen MA, et al. Maximum phonation time: variability and reliability. J Voice. 2010;24(3):281-4. [PubMed ID: 19111437]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2008.10.004.

  • 45.

    Al-Yahya SN, Mohamed Akram MHH, Vijaya Kumar K, Mat Amin SNA, Abdul Malik NA, Mohd Zawawi NA, et al. Maximum Phonation Time Normative Values Among Malaysians and Its Relation to Body Mass Index. J Voice. 2022;36(4):457-63. [PubMed ID: 32861567]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2020.07.015.

  • 46.

    Schwarz J, Spies JP, Bringmann H. Reduced muscle contraction and a relaxed posture during sleep-like Lethargus. Worm. 2012;1(1):12-4. [PubMed ID: 24058817]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3670164]. https://doi.org/10.4161/worm.19499.

  • 47.

    Hammond TH, Zhou R, Hammond EH, Pawlak A, Gray SD. The intermediate layer: a morphologic study of the elastin and hyaluronic acid constituents of normal human vocal folds. J Voice. 1997;11(1):59-66. [PubMed ID: 9075177]. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-1997(97)80024-0.

  • 48.

    Titze IR, Jiang J, Drucker DG. Preliminaries to the body-cover theory of pitch control. J Voice. 1988;1(4):314-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-1997(88)80004-3.

  • 49.

    Butler JE, Hammond TH, Gray SD. Gender-related differences of hyaluronic acid distribution in the human vocal fold. Laryngoscope. 2001;111(5):907-11. [PubMed ID: 11359176]. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005537-200105000-00029.

  • 50.

    Hunter EJ, Tanner K, Smith ME. Gender differences affecting vocal health of women in vocally demanding careers. Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. 2011;36(3):128-36. [PubMed ID: 21722077]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3306615]. https://doi.org/10.3109/14015439.2011.587447.

  • 51.

    Vorperian HK, Kurtzweil SL, Fourakis M, Kent RD, Tillman KK, Austin D. Effect of body position on vocal tract acoustics: Acoustic pharyngometry and vowel formants. J Acoust Soc Am. 2015;138(2):833-45. [PubMed ID: 26328699]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4545056]. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4926563.

  • 52.

    Lee J, Shaiman S, Weismer G. Relationship between tongue positions and formant frequencies in female speakers. J Acoust Soc Am. 2016;139(1):426-40. [PubMed ID: 26827037]. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4939894.

  • 53.

    Di Benedetto M. Vowel representation: Some observations on temporal and spectral properties of the first formant frequency. J Acoust Soc Am. 1989;86(1):55-66. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.398220.

  • 54.

    Sivasankar M, Fisher KV. Oral breathing increases Pth and vocal effort by superficial drying of vocal fold mucosa. J Voice. 2002;16(2):172-81. [PubMed ID: 12150370]. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0892-1997(02)00087-5.

  • 55.

    Fung K, Yoo J, Leeper HA, Hawkins S, Heeneman H, Doyle PC, et al. Vocal function following radiation for non-laryngeal versus laryngeal tumors of the head and neck. Laryngoscope. 2001;111(11 Pt 1):1920-4. [PubMed ID: 11801970]. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005537-200111000-00009.

  • 56.

    Fraser AG. Review article: gastro-oesophageal reflux and laryngeal symptoms. Aliment Pharmacol Ther. 1994;8(3):265-72. [PubMed ID: 7918920]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2036.1994.tb00287.x.

  • 57.

    Hanson DG, Jiang JJ. Diagnosis and management of chronic laryngitis associated with reflux. Am J Med. 2000;108 Suppl 4a:112S-9S. [PubMed ID: 10718463]. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-9343(99)00349-6.

  • 58.

    Sereg-Bahar M, Jansa R, Hocevar-Boltezar I. Voice disorders and gastroesophageal reflux. Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. 2005;30(3-4):120-4. [PubMed ID: 16287651]. https://doi.org/10.1080/14015430500320182.