What Is Left Unsaid About Semantic, Episodic and Working Memory: L2 Cognitive Control Accounts for Individual Differences and Gender Differences

authors:

avatar Behzad Nasirpour 1 , avatar Mohammad Sadegh Bagheri 1 , * , avatar Behnam Jameie 2

Department of English Language, Shiraz Branch, Islamic Azad University, Shiraz, Iran
Department of Anatomy and Neuroscience, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran

how to cite: Nasirpour B, Bagheri M S, Jameie B. What Is Left Unsaid About Semantic, Episodic and Working Memory: L2 Cognitive Control Accounts for Individual Differences and Gender Differences. Thrita. 2018;7(2):e85857. https://doi.org/10.5812/thrita.85857.

Abstract

Background:

Prefrontal cortex (PFC) is believed to manipulate cognitive control and mnemonic process to overcome interference. The interaction among memory, cognitive control, and language learning is still under debate.

Objectives:

This study investigated how episodic, semantic, and working memory (WM) as linguistically dependent retrieval cues can entail the interference process among EFL (English as a foreign language ) individuals.

Methods:

Combining the samples from five testing sectors in Shiraz, Iran, yielded data from 78 adult participants (33 males; 52 EFL independent users; 26 basic users; mean age 34.63). The participants took the test package of selection mechanism measures in resolving interference (SMMRI), including selection in semantic/episodic memory and WM/priming (backward printed reading comprehension). Both descriptive statistics (mean scores, standard deviations) and inferential statistics (Correlations, t-tests, interface resolution (IR)) were performed using SPSS software version 25.0.

Results:

There was a downward trend in the order of magnitude from episodic (OLD-NEW) toward semantic (high-selection) toward WM/priming. Also, there was a moderate/high correlation among the aforementioned memories. There was a significant difference in IR-scores between basic and independent users.

Conclusions:

Hierarchical process of compromises between L2 (Second language) memories indicated that the interactions of different cognitive sub-components played a central executive role. The difference between IR-scores revealed that training on WM tasks, which demanded higher activations in the PFC, could enhance the ability in resolving interference. Retrieval cues have limited concomitant memory capacity that integrate morphological, semantic, and syntactic features. However, men outperformed females in semantic selection, no significant gender difference was seen in episodic selection.

1. Background

In today’s world of interdisciplinary fields, neuroeducation applications have tried to make a vital contribution to how cognitive neuroscience can affect learning because teaching methodology by itself did not seem to make a major breakthrough in what should an individual learn and retrieve (1). It seems that many L2 (Second language) methodologies should consider different multidisciplinary issues such as how far L2 cognitive control encompasses the hierarchical process of compromises between L2 memory pathways? Is there any significant difference among individuals (e.g., males/females) in L2 cognitive control? Accordingly, Thompson-Schill et al. suggest researchers to assess individual differences in cognitive abilities among different developmental groups (e.g., infants, adolescents) since different stages of prefrontal maturation are coupled with different learning opportunities (2).

The interaction between cognitive control and memory is considered within the context of retrieving goal-relevant knowledge from semantic memory, working memory (WM), and priming (3). Evidence shows that semantic memory, working memory and episodic memory encompass selection mechanisms that resolve mnemonic interference (4). Cognitive control mediates semantic retrieval (3). Left mid- and posterior ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) regions are engaged to the extent that semantic decisions require selecting goal-relevant information in the face of competition (5). Regarding episodic memory, selection to overcome interference probably plays a role during both encoding and retrieval (6). Individuals can also facilitate learning by the use of priming sothat they can detect a stimulus based on recent experience with the same stimulus (7).

Language comprehension, problem-solving and other high-level cognitive functions rely on working memory (8). This establishes that the architecture of working memory can be an indispensable part of cognitive control mechanisms (3). Working memory is crucially involved in both native/foreign word learning as well as in the sentence and text comprehension (9). The involvement of working memory in the human language system is not restricted to word learning since working memory may be involved in the integration of individual words into coherent sentences and discourse representations (9). Daneman and Carpenter discovered that there is a correlation between working memory span measures and reading comprehension (10). The cognitive mechanism is necessary to realize complex expressions from simpler ones consisting of three levels, including form, semantics, and syntax (11). Complex meanings are assembled bottom-up from the meanings of the lexical elements by the combinatorial machinery of syntax (12). Unification operations take place at the syntactic level; furthermore, at the semantic and phonological levels, the lexical elements are integrated into larger structures (13). Three functional components of language processing i.e., memory, unification, and control (MUC) are utilized in both language production and language comprehension (13).

However syntactic and semantic features were investigated in sentence comprehension (14), perceptual priming and morphophonemic features remained recondite in L2 sentence comprehension.

2. Objectives

This study aimed to offer a comprehensive depiction of individual EFL learners’ semantic/episodic memory and WM/priming.

3. Methods

3.1. Study Design and Participants

Five different universities and sectors where English is the medium or studied as a foreign language were negotiated and selected through quota sampling from Shiraz, Iran in early 2018. The participants of the study are comprised of 78 (33 males; 52 EFL independent users; 26 EFL basic users; mean age 34.63) graduate/post graduate students of universities, EFL learners and non-students, namely Agriculture College of Shiraz University (ACSU); Islamic Azad University, Shiraz Branch (IAU); South Industrial Management Institute (SIMI); Fars Regional Water Company (FRWC), and Pooräb Fars Engineering Consulting Company (PFECC). The participants were not chosen based on their academic scores, intelligence, sex, racial segregation, etc.

This project aimed at the subjects at the level of basic users (A1 & A2) and independent users (B1 & B2) based on the CEFR (common European framework of reference for languages) description (in English). Identification of the proficiency level of the participants was based on (1) IELTS (international English language testing system) test reports which include CEFR level, and (2) IELTS mock test (2018). Most of the participants had taken the test between two to four weeks before the study. The participants’ overall CEFR levels fell within the scope of basic users (26 subjects) and independent users (52 subjects).

3.2. Data Collection

The package of instruments to assess selection retrievals within the domain of episodic, semantic, and working memories, is selection mechanism measures in resolving interference (SMMRI), which will be discussed below.

(1) In order to determine the participants’ semantic interference, the revised version of Thompson-Schill’s task (5) was utilized. The task was categorized into low-selection and high-selection tables. Task subjects were shown concrete nouns (word length ranges from 3 to 9) chosen from Davies and Gardner’s frequency dictionary (15). The frequency for the lemma ranges was from 5800 to 69223 and the dispersion from 0.83 to 0.95. The participants were required to generate semantically related verbs to measure the level of semantic interference in VLPFC. Forty nouns were presented in total, which included 20 high and 20 low selection items.

(2) Within the domain of episodic memory, selection to overcome interference likely plays a role during both encoding and retrieval. The revised version of Dolan and Fletcher’s PET (positron emission tomography) study was used to measure neural responses during the encoding of word pairs (16). This manipulated the extent to which prior learning interfered with the current encoding. The participants firstly studied a list of word pairs; next, two lists of word pairs (New-New/Old-Old and Old-New) were studied, containing repeated pairs and completely novel pairs (Old-Old/New-New), and the pairs that partially overlapped with previously studied pairs (Old-New).

(3) In order to determine the participants’ WM-Priming memory associations (complex span paradigm), Paller and Gross’s visual word-form priming test (17), Daneman and Carpenter’s Reading span task (10), and Nasirpour’s WM/priming RC (reading comprehension) task (18) were inspired and integrated. This test consisted of four reading comprehensions (10 RC questions; sentence length ranged from 15 to 18 words; each sentence had an underlying phrase marker consisting of one S) which were printed backward. It should be noted that the reading comprehensions were chosen from Bagheri and Tavakoli’s TOEFL actual tests (19). The participants were supposed to read the four reading comprehensions and make the choice a, b, c or d. The total time for the WM-Priming RC tasks was 10 minutes.

3.3. Ethical Considerations

However, the informed consent was obtained from all participants before the study, the nature and purpose of the research were again explained to them.

3.4. Statistical Analysis

Both descriptive statistics (mean, median, standard deviations) and inferential statistics (correlations, t-tests, interface resolution (IR), and error rate (ER)) were carried out to determine the magnitude of the interference. Analysis was performed using SPSS software version 25.0. A P ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

In order to estimate the participants’ interface resolution (IR), the calculation of Thompson-Schill et al. and Persson et al. were scrutinized (5, 20). IR scores can be estimated by subtracting error rates (ER) for low interface from ER’s for high interface (5, 20). The tasks were categorized into the low-interface and high-interface for the two subtypes of memory (semantic/episodic memory).

4. Results

The mean scores of episodic memory, semantic memory and WM/priming RC tasks fell at different levels in SMMRI tests (Table 1). Scores were presented with percent for a better comparison. The highest memory score was semantic memory (low-selection) with a mean of 75 (SD = 21.33). The lowest memory score was WM/priming in RC, with a mean of 37.7 (SD = 2.51).

A downward trend in the order of magnitude (in mean and median) was seen from episodic (OLD-NEW) toward semantic (high-selection) toward WM/priming among basic users. Similarly, another downward trend in the order of magnitude was seen from semantic (high-selection) to WM/priming (in RC) among independent users (Table 2).

Table 1.

Median, Mean and SD of Selection Mechanism in L2 Cognitive Control

NMinMaxMedianMean ± SD (%)
Episodic memory: Old-Old7701005054 ± 24.06
Episodic memory: New-New77201007069 ± 19.57
Episodic memory: Old-New770905050.5 ± 19.25
Semantic memory: Low-selection77151008075 ± 21.33
Semantic memory: High-selection777915858.2 ± 20.10
WM/priming RC tasks77010437.7 ± 2.51
Valid N (list wise)77
Table 2.

Median, Mean and SD of Selection Mechanism in Cognitive Control Between Basic and Independent Users

NMinMaxMedianMean ± SD (%)
Episodic memory:Old-Old
Basic users26201006059 ± 21.66
Independent users5201005052 ± 24.89
Episodic memory: New-New
Basic users2620857067 ± 19.43
Independent users52201007070 ± 19.54
Episodic memory: Old-New
Basic users260904546 ± 22.65
Independent users5230905053 ± 17.00
Semantic memory: Low- selection
Basic users2615855053 ± 18.81
Independent users52501009085 ± 12.90
Semantic memory: High- selection
Basic users267634038 ± 15.45
Independent users5233917168 ± 13.63
WM/priming in RC
Basic users2606216.2 ± 1.87
Independent users52010648.5 ± 2.18

IR scores can be estimated by subtracting error rates (ER) for low-interface from ER’s for high-interface. Subjects’ medians were used to minimize the effect of extreme values on the distribution.

A significant tendency for episodic interference was seen among EFL basic users rather than EFL independent users (Table 2). A significant baseline difference was evident between the groups and measured by the ER difference between NEW-NEW and OLD-NEW. However, both groups showed a significant interference in pairs that partially overlapped with previously studied pairs (OLD-NEW).

(1) IR (basic users) = ER for OLD-OLD - ER for OLD-NEW = 40 - 55 = - 15

(2) IR (basic users) = ER for NEW-NEW - ER for OLD-NEW = 30 - 55 = - 25

(3) IR (independent users) = ER for OLD-OLD - ER for OLD-NEW = 50 - 50 = 0

(4) IR (independent users) = ER for NEW-NEW - ER for OLD-NEW = 30 - 50 = - 20

For semantic interference analyses, participants’ medians were used to minimize the effect of extreme values on the distribution. EFL basic and independent users’ performances are shown on the verb generate task in Table 2, where improvement in the speed of performance is generally evident. A significant interference was seen between EFL basic and independent users. Moreover, both groups showed a significant semantic interference (in the interference scores) in high interference condition which lacked a clear dominant response and low interference condition, which had one dominant response.

(5) IR (basic users) = ER for low-selection - ER for high-selection = 47 – 62 = - 15

(6) IR (independent users) = ER for low-selection - ER for high-selection = 15 – 32 = - 17

The results presented in Table 3 indicate that there was a moderate correlation between episodic memory (OLD-NEW) and semantic memory (high-selection) and WM/priming RC tasks (P = 0.000; P = 0.023). The correlation coefficient was significant for both episodic memory (OLD-NEW)/semantic memory and WM/priming RC tests. In this regard, there was a moderate correlation between episodic memory (OLD-NEW) and semantic memory (high-selection). Likewise, episodic memory (OLD-NEW) correlated with WM/priming RC tasks.

Table 3.

Pearson Correlations Between Episodic/Semantic Memory and WM/Priming RC Task

Semantic Memory: Low-SelectionSemantic Memory: High-SelectionWM/Priming RC Tasks
Episodic memory: Old-Old
Pearson correlation0.077- 0.0390.084
Sig. (2-tailed)0.5060.7380.469
Episodic memory: New-New
Pearson correlation0.2220.1680.054
Sig. (2-tailed)0.0520.1450.642
Episodic memory: Old-New
Pearson correlation0.447a0.486a0.258b
Sig. (2-tailed)0.0000.0000.023
N777777

Table 4 illustrates that there was a moderate correlation (P = 0.023) between WM/priming (in RC) and episodic memory (OLD-NEW). More interestingly, there was a high correlation (P = 0.000) between WM/priming (in RC) and semantic memory (both low and high selections).

Table 4.

Pearson Correlations Between WM/Priming (in RC) and Episodic/Semantic Memory (SMMRI)

WM/Priming RC TasksEpisodic Memory OLD-NEWSemantic Memory Low SelectionSemantic Memory High Selection
Pearson correlation0.258a0.677b0.649b
Sig. (2-tailed)0.0230.0000.000
N777777

In order to see whether there was a significant difference between EFL basic and independent users, Independent Samples test and Descriptive Statistics were carried out (Table 5). There was a significant difference between the means of EFL basic and independent users in semantic memory (low/high selection) and WM/priming (P = 0.000). The mean scores of independent users were greater than those of basic users regarding semantic memory and WM/priming.

Table 5.

Descriptive Statistics and t-Test Results Combined on Semantic and WM/Primingi Between EFL Basic and Independent Users

NMean ± SDFSig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed)Mean Difference
Semantic: Low-selection7.270.009
Basic users2553.40 ± 19.18- 9.08740.000- 32.67
Independent users5186.08 ± 12.01- 7.79330.000- 32.67
Semantic: High-selection1.220.273
Basic users2537.92 ± 15.74- 9.11740.000- 30.78
Independent users5168.71 ± 12.82- 8.49400.000- 30.78
WM/Priming RC tasks0.4250.516
Basic users251.68 ± 1.88- 6.58740.000- 3.26
Independent users514.94 ± 2.09- 6.82520.000- 3.26

However, there was no significant difference between EFL basic and independent users regarding episodic interference (Table 6).

Table 6.

t-Test Results: Episodic Interference Between EFL Basic and Independent Users

FSig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed)Mean Difference
Episodic: Old-Old0.0080.929
Equal variances assumed1.23740.2227.23
Equal variances not assumed1.28530.2047.23
Episodic:New-New0.0010.971
Equal variances assumed- 0.72740.471- 3.49
Equal variances not assumed- 0.72520.473- 3.49
Episodic: Old-New3.070.083
Equal variances assumed- 1.75740.083- 8.14
Equal variances not assumed- 1.59380.119- 8.14

To see whether there was a significant difference between males and females in PI in cognitive control, independent samples test and descriptive statistics were performed for EFL basic and independent users, respectively. Firstly, information in descriptive statistics and independent samples test was combined in Table 7 for EFL basic users. The result indicated that the differences were significant between the means of males and females in semantic memory (low/high selection) (0.045 and 0.013, respectively). The mean score of male basic users was greater than that of females regarding semantic memory (low/high selection).

Table 7.

Descriptive Statistics and t-Test Results Combined in Semantic/Episodic/WM Interference by EFL Basic User Males and Females (N = 26)

NMean ± SDFSig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed)Mean Difference
Episodic: Old-Old
Male103.00 ± 11.598.940.0060.69240.4946.12
Female1656.88 ± 26.190.8122.20.4236.12
Episodic: New-New
Male1069.50 ± 16.230.870.3590.56240.5764.50
Female1665.00 ± 21.520.60230.5514.50
Episodic: Old-New
Male1056.00 ± 20.110.090.7591.91240.06816.62
Female1639.38 ± 22.351.9620.80.06316.62
Semantic:Low- selection
Male1062.50 ± 15.500.340.5622.11240.04515.00
Female1647.50 ± 18.792.21220.03815.00
Semantic: High- selection
Male1046.90 ± 12.150.950.3392.67240.01314.90
Female1632.00 ± 14.772.79220.01114.90
WM/priming in RC
Male102.40 ± 1.830.010.9161.75240.0921.27
Female161.13 ± 1.781.7418.80.0981.27

Secondly, information in descriptive statistics and independent samples test was combined in Table 8 for EFL independent user males and females. There was a significant difference between the means of males and females in semantic interference (high selection) (P = 0.021).

Table 8.

Descriptive Statistics and t-Test Results Combined in Semantic/Episodic/WM Interference by Independent User Males and Females (N = 52)

NMean ± SDFSig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed)Mean Difference
Episodic: Old-Old
Male2350.22 ± 23.080.300.583- 0.43500.664- 3.05
Female2953.28 ± 26.56- 0.4449.50.659- 3.05
Episodic: New-New
Male2375.22 ± 18.300.220.6381.71500.0939.18
Female2966.03 ± 19.831.7348.80.0909.18
Episodic: Old-New
Male2357.83 ± 14.122.940.0921.75500.0858.17
Female2949.66 ± 18.411.81500.0768.17
Semantic: Low- selection
Male2387.61 ± 10.433.060.0861.10500.2733.98
Female2983.62 ± 14.511.1549.60.2553.98
Semantic: High- selection
Male2372.87 ± 9.4211.650.0012.38500.0218.69
Female2964.17 ± 15.292.5147.40.0158.69
WM/priming in RC tasks
Male235.13 ± 1.683.810.0560.83500.4080.51
Female294.62 ± 2.510.8748.80.3870.51

5. Discussion

5.1. Sequential Executive Functions, L2 Cognitive Processes and Individual Differences

To have a better understanding of EFL learners’ cognitive control mechanisms, this study is premised on the ideas that a dynamic interaction exists between L2 cognitive control and memory pathways; in addition, a hierarchical evolutionary process exists in episodic, semantic and working memories.

As it is evident in Table 3, there was a moderate correlation between Episodic memory (OLD-NEW) and Semantic memory (High/Low-selection). In both Dolan and Fletcher’s experiment (16) and this study, episodic memory (OLD-NEW) was the lowest memory score, which shows a critical emphasis on necessary semantic processing for the formation of new category-exemplar associations. Dolan and Fletcher postulated that this manipulation must elicit a degree of interference from previously encoded pairs (i.e., proactive interference) (16). Moreover, there was a correlation (P = 0.023) between WM/priming (in RC) and episodic memory (OLD-NEW). Similar to high interference attributed to semantic and working memory, PI in episodic tasks would also be engaged in selection processes for successful task execution. Jonides et al. broached the subject there might be some anatomical overlap in the neural substrates recruited by working memory and episodic memory tasks (21); and recent neuroimaging studies showed similar left inferior frontal (IFG) activations during high interference trials in both tasks (20). The findings revealed that L2 cognitive control is the hierarchical process of evolutionary compromises because word meaning should be inculcated in the learners’ minds and then it can be assembled into compound meaning.

Intensive performance of one cognitive process with other processes (e.g., in PFC) which share executive functions may cause cognitive fatigue if a selection process is involved in multiple memory domains (20, 22) or subjects are not cognitively matured to select goal-relevant representations from among competitors (e.g., basic users). This was detected in the successive memory tasks subjects carried out; in other words, a downward tendency was seen from episodic (OLD-NEW) toward semantic (high-selection) toward WM/priming (in RC) which indicated that the interactions of different cognitive sub-components had a central executive function (Table 2). The result was in line with Van der Linden et al. (22) that multiple executive processes can result in reduced performance on the other tasks. Thus, the shared executive processes can include shared cognitive processes for basic users; therefore, contribute to cognitive fatigue.

The result (Table 4) suggests that high interference conditions of the WM, semantic memory (high selections), and episodic memory OLD-NEW are mediated by a common cognitive control mechanism. This was in line with the experiments conducted by Persson et al. (20). Moreover, the difference between IR-scores, response time (RT), and error rate (ER) among basic and independent users revealed that training on WM task, which demand higher activations in the PFC, can enhance the ability to resolve interference. This was in agreement with both Szmalec et al. and Persson et al. (9, 20).

5.2. Semantic Unification and Cue-based Parsing

To manipulate the morphophonemic (perceptual priming), syntactic, and semantic properties to create interference conditions, the researchers controlled RC sentences in the form of an anti-saccade task. In this inhibition task, participants must move their eyes away from a visual routine (left alignment), decoding the words which were printed backward (i.e., high vs. low morphophonemic interference × low vs. high semantic interference × low vs. high syntactic interference). This anti-saccade study focused on L2 cognitive improvements in the ability to select between competing bootstraps, inhibit inappropriate morphophonemic shapes, rearrange the response to the previous form, retrieve the new form from memory, and monitor the performance.

The memory score was rather low in WM/priming RC tasks (Tables 1 and 2). Due to the perceptual priming challenges (in backward printed words), linguistically dependent cues could strain learners’ recollection and this proved that retrieval cues might have limited concomitant memory capacity in the sentence comprehension. The mean of WM/priming in RC tasks also revealed that priming-related activation needs time to reprocess; in addition, basic users’ previous exposure to the stimuli seemed to be shallow and was not strong enough to produce experience-based activation in a form of LTM. To Wagner et al., perceptual priming is not sensitive to the level of semantic elaboration during initial processing, whereas conceptual priming, which is modality-independent, is sensitive to semantic features of a stimulus during the study (3). Thus especially among basic users in this study, the tuning of semantic memory did not sharpen their conceptual representations since the initial retrieval of relevant stimulus features did not help to inhibit less relevant features. This led to high competition and interference.

There was a bottleneck with a lower processing rate in WM/priming (RC tasks) for both EFL basic and independent users. It was detected that each linguistically dependent cue had its own interference and depended on the considerable degree of overlap between the dependent memories, which were involved in perceptual priming, automatic retrieval, post-retrieval selection (in RC), and the representations generated for each processing step. Cue-based retrievals need to be processed in the semantic unification system to infer stored semantic knowledge through a top-down process. According to Oberauer and Kliegl’s study, participants must alternate between encoding memory cues and processing other information in any complex span paradigm (i.e., WM paradigm) (23). Thus the binding mechanism must use synchronous firing from deciphering cues to post-retrieval selection to semantic/syntactic interaction which challenges memory demand. The results of WM/priming RC tasks, which showed a sharp downward trend in the scores, were in consistent with Oberauer and Kliegl’s study (23). All in all, items did not seem to be independent in sentence comprehension, thus as Van Dyke and McElree broach, the cue-driven associative retrieval mechanism, are needed to be synthesized in sentence comprehension (24).

5.3. Gender Difference

The results of this study showed that there was no significant difference between the mean scores of males and females using episodic selection (OLD-OLD/NEW-NEW/OLD-NEW). However, the findings in another study revealed that females had a lower spatial ability (in particular, mental rotation ability) than males (25). This implies that the nature of episodic memory should be clearly defined in each study. Furthermore, there was no significant difference between the males and females in WM/priming (RC tasks); this partly disagree with the findings of Upadhayay and Guragain who observed that male cognitive functions (attentional, perceptual, and executive functions) were different in comparison to those of the female pre-ovulatory phase (26).

Unlike the present study, which experimented selection mechanisms to compare the genders in L2 semantic selection (low/high selection), Wirth et al. used event-related potentials (ERP) to compare the genders in lower and higher order semantic processing (controlled semantic analysis) during the passive reading of semantically related- and unrelated word pairs (27). Their finding indicated that the initial lexical-semantic access was similar in men and women; however, the genders differed in higher order semantic processing. The probable reasons for a better semantic selection (high selection) among males in this study can be attributed to males’ priority to visual/spatial perception (28), female weakness during the pre-ovulatory phase/menstrual cycle (26), etc.

5.4. Conclusion

The findings revealed that individuals’ L2 cognitive control is a hierarchical process, in which word meaning should be inculcated in their minds and thereby can be assembled into the compound meaning. The findings of independent users revealed that practice, exposure and, experience, which involve executive functions, can enhance performance on subsequent cognitive subparts. As for EFL basic users, signs of fatigue might be related to a number of factors, including the extent of practice, shallow recollection, proactive interference (PI), impaired cue-based parsing, weak semantic unification, etc. To avoid hasty conclusions, inappropriate analogies between male and female genders should not be made because a number of factors such as the nature of controlled vs. automatic process, the nature of episodic memory, sex hormone profiles (e.g., estrogen), menstrual cycle, female/male menopause, and so on can influence the results of L2 cognitive control. In delving into individual/group mental map, for upgrading any educational qualifications in L2 contexts, cognitive-minded discipline is highly recommended.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Mehta A. "Neuroeducation" emerges as insights into brain development, learning abilities grow. The DANA Foundation; 2009.

  • 2.

    Thompson-Schill SL, D'Esposito M, Kan IP. Effects of repetition and competition on activity in left prefrontal cortex during word generation. Neuron. 1999;23(3):513-22. [PubMed ID: 10433263]. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80804-1.

  • 3.

    Wagner AD, Bunge SA, Badre D. Cognitive control, semantic memory, and priming: Contributions from prefrontal cortex. Cogn Neurosci. 2004;3:709-25.

  • 4.

    Badre D, Wagner AD. Frontal lobe mechanisms that resolve proactive interference. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(12):2003-12. [PubMed ID: 15788702]. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi075.

  • 5.

    Thompson-Schill SL, D'Esposito M, Aguirre GK, Farah MJ. Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: A reevaluation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94(26):14792-7. [PubMed ID: 9405692]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC25116]. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.94.26.14792.

  • 6.

    Race EA, Kuhl BA, Badre D, Wagner AD. The dynamic interplay between cognitive control and memory. Cogn Neurosci. 2009:705-23.

  • 7.

    Paller KA, Squire LR. Biology of memory. In: Sadock BJ, Sadock VA, Ruiz P, editors. Kaplan and Sadock's comprehensive textbook of psychiatry. 9th ed. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2009.

  • 8.

    Just MA, Carpenter PA. A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychol Review. 1992;99(1):122. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122.

  • 9.

    Szmalec A, Brysbaert M, Duyck W. Working memory and (second) language processing. In: Altarriba J, editor. Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches. Cambridge University Press; 2012. p. 74-94. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139035279.004.

  • 10.

    Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verb Learn Verb Be. 1980;19(4):450-66. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6.

  • 11.

    Jackendoff R. Precis of Foundations of language: brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Behav Brain Sci. 2003;26(6):651-65. discussion 666-707. [PubMed ID: 15377127]. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198270126.001.0001.

  • 12.

    Jackendoff RS. The architecture of the language faculty. Unites State: MIT Press; 1997.

  • 13.

    Hagoort P, Baggio G, Willems RM. Semantic unification. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The cognitive neurosciences. 4th ed. MIT Press; 2009. p. 819-36.

  • 14.

    Tabor W, Hutchins S. Evidence for self-organized sentence processing: Digging-in effects. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004;30(2):431-50. [PubMed ID: 14979816]. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.431.

  • 15.

    Davies M, Gardner D. A frequency dictionary of contemporary American English: Word sketches, collocates and thematic lists. Routledge; 2013. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203880883.

  • 16.

    Dolan RJ, Fletcher PC. Dissociating prefrontal and hippocampal function in episodic memory encoding. Nature. 1997;388(6642):582-5. [PubMed ID: 9252188]. https://doi.org/10.1038/41561.

  • 17.

    Paller KA, Gross M. Brain potentials associated with perceptual priming vs explicit remembering during the repetition of visual word-form. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36(6):559-71. [PubMed ID: 9705066]. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00132-2.

  • 18.

    Nasirpour B. The assessment of organizational intelligence among experts and engineers in Fars Regional Water Company: Implementation of meta-knowledge management. Iran's Ministry of Energy, Practical Research Committee of Fars Regional Water Co; 2018.

  • 19.

    Bagheri MS, Tavakoli F. TOEFL reader's digest in a flash. Azaran; 2001.

  • 20.

    Persson J, Welsh KM, Jonides J, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Cognitive fatigue of executive processes: Interaction between interference resolution tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(7):1571-9. [PubMed ID: 17227678]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC1876692]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.007.

  • 21.

    Jonides J, Smith EE, Marshuetz C, Koeppe RA, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(14):8410-3. [PubMed ID: 9653200]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC20989]. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.14.8410.

  • 22.

    van der Linden D, Frese M, Meijman TF. Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: Effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2003;113(1):45-65. [PubMed ID: 12679043]. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00150-6.

  • 23.

    Oberauer K, Kliegl R. A formal model of capacity limits in working memory. J Mem Lang. 2006;55(4):601-26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.009.

  • 24.

    Van Dyke JA. Cue-dependent interference in comprehension. J Mem Lang. 2011;65(3):247-63. [PubMed ID: 21927535]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3171743]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.05.002.

  • 25.

    Guillem F, Mograss M. Gender differences in memory processing: Evidence from event-related potentials to faces. Brain Cogn. 2005;57(1):84-92. [PubMed ID: 15629219]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.026.

  • 26.

    Upadhayay N, Guragain S. Comparison of cognitive functions between male and female medical students: A pilot study. J Clin Diagn Res. 2014;8(6):BC12-5. [PubMed ID: 25120970]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4129348]. https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2014/7490.4449.

  • 27.

    Wirth M, Horn H, Koenig T, Stein M, Federspiel A, Meier B, et al. Sex differences in semantic processing: event-related brain potentials distinguish between lower and higher order semantic analysis during word reading. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(9):1987-97. [PubMed ID: 17116651]. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl121.

  • 28.

    Bryant DJ. A spatial representation system in humans. Am Psychol Assoc. 1992;31:74-98.