The Common and Specific Role of Emotional Factors in Current Cravings and Thoughts-Imagination of Consumption in Opioid Dependent Patients

authors:

avatar Behzad Rigi Kooteh ORCID 1 , avatar Behrooz Dolatshahi ORCID 2 , avatar Nour-Mohammad Bakhshani ORCID 3 , avatar Yazdan Naderi Rajeh ORCID 4 , *

Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Medicine, Children and Adolescents Health Research Center, Resistant Tuberculosis Institute, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, IR Iran
Substance Abuse and Dependence Research Center, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, IR Iran
Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Medicine, Children and Adolescents Health Research Center, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, IR Iran
Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, IR Iran

how to cite: Rigi Kooteh B, Dolatshahi B, Bakhshani N, Naderi Rajeh Y. The Common and Specific Role of Emotional Factors in Current Cravings and Thoughts-Imagination of Consumption in Opioid Dependent Patients. Int J High Risk Behav Addict. 2021;10(4):e115515. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijhrba.115515.

Abstract

Background:

Craving has an important place in the structure of the literature on consumption disorders.

Objectives:

The present research investigates the relationship between emotional factors (such as positive and negative effects, emotion regulation, emotion regulation strategies, emotional processing, emotional stimulation, attention to emotional stimuli, emotional separation, and emotional response) with consumption craving.

Patients and Methods:

The statistical population of current descriptive-correlational research consisted of all opium-dependent patients living in medium-term accommodation centers (camps) in Zahedan, Iran, from 2018 to 2019. Among 21 allowed accommodation centers, 12 were randomly selected. A purposive sampling method was used to select 243 patients. For collecting data, multiple questionnaires were used. Pearson correlation coefficient test and stepwise multivariate regression were used for analyzing the data. Data were analyzed using SPSS-16 software.

Results:

The results showed that among emotional factors, emotional excitement could only predict 0.020 of variations in instant craving, and other emotional factors did not have much explanatory power. However, to predict drug carvings and desires, more emotional variables (emotional arousal, emotional processing, affection, emotion regulation, and emotional separation) were entered into the equation, and the set could predict 0.156% of drug carving.

Conclusions:

Cultural factors play an important role in the expression of emotion, and it is important to consider them in the field of drug prevention. Concurrent attention is needed to be paid to the biological, psychological, and social dimensions. In addition, concurrent treatment can lead to interdisciplinary collaboration.

1. Background

Opioids contain compounds that can interact with opioid receptors in the brain. Prolonged and unsupervised (abuse) use can lead to opioid dependence and other health problems (1). The opioid epidemic is a significant public health concern in the world. In recent years, the use of opioids with non-medical prescriptions has been increasing worldwide, which sets the stage for further risks (2).

Addiction is always closely related to human emotions, which helps to better understand the causes of abuse, maintenance, and relapse (3). Emotions, emotional states, and emotional-physiological responses are classics that are interpreted based on the history of the organism and the context (4). There is a connection between the opioid system of the brain and human emotions. Stimulation of approach-oriented emotions (anger and pleasure), avoidance-oriented emotions (fear and sadness), social bonding, and dependency behavior are influenced by the opioid system of the brain (5).

Emotions are influential in many important concepts in substance abuse research, such as trait anxiety, inhibition, reaction to environmental cues, cognitive regulation, mood, and behavioral motivation (6). Negative emotions trigger addictive behavior. Negative emotions such as sadness, anxiety, and negative mood increase the use of addictive substances and, in case of sadness, can inadvertently increase cravings (7). Emotion regulation, as an emotional component, predicts addictive behaviors such as alcohol and drug use, gambling, video game addiction, and problematic Internet use and is useful for preventive and curative interventions (8). Uncontrolled anger is also a stimulus for re-use and relapse among opioid-dependent patients (9, 10).

Drug addicts suffer from rejection of emotional responses, difficulty performing purposeful behaviors, difficulty controlling impulses, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and lack of emotional clarity, and this provides the basis for continued use (11). Emotional factors are actually risk factors for drug use, and chronic drug use can lead to more emotional disorders with an effect on the reward system, motivation, and stress. On the other hand, perturbations in these systems lead to emotional processing of drug-related symptoms in exchange for natural rewards that maintain addictive behavior by increasing cravings (12).

2. Objectives

Considering the importance of emotions in relapse and craving, this study investigated the specific and common role of emotional factors with craving and its thoughts and fantasies in opioid dependent patients in Zahedan, Iran.

3. Patients and Methods

The design of present study is descriptive-correlational, and the research variables are measured Concurrently. The statistical population of the study included all opioid dependents in Zahedan living in medium-term accommodation centers (camps) in 2018. All subjects were male with an age range of 20 - 44 years. Sampling was done in several stages. First, sampling was done randomly. Thus, at first, the residential centers licensed by the Welfare Organization of Sistan and Baluchestan Province (Zahedan city) were identified by referring to the welfare office in the city. A total of 12 accommodation centers were selected from 21 accommodation centers. Then, purposeful sampling was performed, so that by referring to the patients’ records in the accommodation center and the type of drugs used and the criteria for entering and leaving the research, the sample was determined. Finally, out of 700 opium-dependent patients in 21 accommodation centers, 243 samples were identified according to Morgan and Freeman’s table.

Inclusion criteria were diagnosis of opioid addiction, male gender, age range of 20 - 44 years, and being literate (read and write). Exclusion criteria were concomitant use of methadone maintenance therapy, diagnosis of other substance use disorders, having physical pain, history of hospitalization in a psychiatric hospital, schizophrenia, and receiving antipsychotic drugs, and unwillingness to participate in the research.

3.1. Desires for Drug Questionnaire

This 14-item questionnaire assesses the current craving (13) and is scored by a seven-point Likert scale. Internal consistency for the three test subscales calculated by Cronbach’s alpha is 0.81, 0.84, and 0.79, respectively (13). The total Cronbach’s alpha value for Iranian opium users was 0.96. (14).

3.2. Measuring the Craving to Use Drugs

This questionnaire was developed (15) in Iran and contains 20 items that measure the amount of thoughts and fantasies about substances and temptations that are experienced after withdrawal. The internal consistency of the questionnaire was obtained in terms of Cronbach's alpha (0.94) (15).

3.3. The Emotion Regulation Difficulty Scale

The components of this 36-item scale (16) include emotional rejection, difficulty in performing purposeful behavior, difficulty in controlling impulse, lack of emotional awareness, limited access to emotional regulation strategies, and lack of emotional clarity (17). Cronbach’s alpha for this questionnaire was 0.92 in Iran.

3.4. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire

This questionnaire consists of 10 questions, and the internal consistency for re-evaluation was 0.79, and repression was 0.73 (18). Hasani (19) reported a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.79 for re-evaluation.

3.5. The Positive and Negative Emotion Scale

This scale has 20 items (20). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the positive emotion subscale was 0.88, and a negative emotion was 0.87. The reliability of the eight-week retest was reported to be 0.68 for the positive emotion subscale and 0.71 for the negative emotion.

3.6. The Emotional Processing Scale

This questionnaire is based on the Likert scale. The reliability according to Cronbach’s alpha for subscales was 0.84 (21). In Iran, Cronbach’s alpha was reported to be 0.95 (22).

3.7. The Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire

This questionnaire was developed by Giganc in 2010 (23). The Cronbach’s alpha in the 14-item short form was 0.76, and the correlation between the short and long forms of this questionnaire was 0.94 (23).

3.8. The Scale of Emotional Arousability

This scale was developed by Britoit in 1987 (24). This scale is rated on a Likert scale from 1 to 5. The reliability calculated by Cronbach’s alpha for all components was 0.83 (24).

3.9. Emotional Reactivity Subscale

In the study by Skowron and Schmitt (25), the reliability of the revised form with Cronbach’s alpha for the whole scale was 0.92, and the homogeneity coefficient for the emotional response subscale was 0.89.

3.10. The Emotional Cutoff Subscale

This tool is one of the subscales of the Differentiation Questionnaire. It was first designed and executed in 1998 by Skowron and Friedlander with 43 items. It was then revised in 2003 by Skowron and Schmitt, and the questionnaire was designed by Jackson with 46 terms and four factors based on Bowen’s theory (25). The reliability of the questionnaire obtained by Skowron and Schmitt (25) for the emotional separation subscale was 0.84. In Rafi’s research (26), the value of the whole scale reliability was 0.90.

Statistical analysis was performed at both descriptive and inferential levels. Statistical indicators such as mean, standard deviation, frequency, etc. were used to evaluate descriptive data, and Pearson correlation coefficient test and stepwise multivariate regression were used for inferential data.

4. Results

The job status of participants was freelancer (n = 168, 69.1%), unemployed (n = 57 23.5%), and employees (n = 13, 5.3%). In terms of marital status, 49.8% (n = 121) were single and 45.3% (n = 110) were married. Regarding education status, the highest frequency was related to under diploma (n = 149, 61.3%), and the lowest frequency was related to postgraduate and higher education (n = 2, 0.8%). The average age of the participants was 30.16 years, with a standard deviation of 5.92. The age range of the subjects was 18 - 44 years.

According to Table 1, there was a direct relationship between emotional factors and current cravings in opioid-dependent patients.

Table 1.

Pearson Correlation Coefficient Between Emotional Factors and Current Craving in Opioid-Dependent Patients

VariableAffectionEmotion RegulationEmotion IntelligenceEmotion ProcessingEmotion IrritabilityCognitive Emotion RegulationEmotional ReactivityEmotional Cutoff
Current cravings0.05-0.03-0.050.13 a0.14 a0.030.06-0.04

As Table 2 shows, there was a significant relationship between all emotional factors (except emotion regulation, emotion intelligence, and emotional reactivity) with current craving. Stepwise regression was used to investigate the predictive role of emotional factors in the current cravings of opioid-dependent patients.

Table 2.

Pearson Correlation Coefficient Between Emotional Factors and Consumption Thoughts in Opioid-Dependent Patients

VariableAffectionEmotion RegulationEmotion IntelligenceEmotion ProcessingEmotion IrritabilityCognitive Emotion RegulationEmotional ReactivityEmotional Cutoff
Thoughts consumption0.20 a0.03-0.020.27 a0.28 a0.23 a0.110.18 a

According to the results of Table 3, emotional irritability could only predict 0.020 of the changes related to current consumption cravings, and other emotional factors did not have such explanatory power and did not enter the equation.

Table 3.

Regression Analysis of Emotional Factors in Relation to Current Craving for Use in Opioid-Dependent Patients a

Criterion VariableStepPredictive VariableRR2R2 AdjustFPBBetaP
Current cravings1Emotion irritability0.140.020.014.930.020.330.1420.027

Stepwise regression was also used to investigate the predictive role of emotional factors in the thoughts and fantasies of opioid-dependent patients.

Regression analysis of emotional factors in relation to opioid consumption thoughts have been presented in Table 4.

Table 4.

Regression Analysis of Emotional Factors in Relation to Opioid Consumption Thoughts

Criterion Variable/Step/Predictive VariableRR2R2 AdjustFPBetaP
Thoughts consumption
1
Emotion irritability0.280.080.0721.60.0010.280.001
2
Emotion irritability0.370.110.115.10.0010.270.001
Emotional cutoff0.010.005
3
Emotion irritability0.370.130.1212.80.0010.20.002
Emotional cutoff0.160.006
Emotion processing0.180.007
4
Emotion irritability0.390.150.1411.10.0010.160.015
Emotional cutoff0.160.007
Emotion processing0.180.006
Affection0.140.023
5
Emotion irritability0.410.170.159.90.0010.130.045
Emotional cutoff0.120.037
Emotion processing0.230.170.007
Affection0.140.018
Emotion regulation0.160.130.036

5. Discussion

The results of the present study showed that emotional factors can predict the current craving for consumption and the related thoughts and fantasies. Emotional factors predicted most consumption thoughts and fantasies and had the most relationship. Results are consistent with previous studies (27, 28).

Craving has always been influenced by several factors (29). Etiology of cravings also emphasizes various other factors such as interpersonal, behavioral, psychological, and bio-genetic factors (16).

In addition to these factors, we can mention the factors of mindfulness, cognition, behavior, and emotion in the discussion of craving (30). Therefore, emotion and related factors can explain only part of the variance, and other factors can play a role. This was one of the reasons that emotional factors had less predictive power in the present study. Substance abuse and addiction is a complex disorder associated with biological, psychological, social, and spiritual causes. The complexity of biological, psychological, social, economic, political, and cultural factors has made this problem one of the most complex individual, family, and social problems (31). People with addiction have difficulty expressing their emotions due to their inability to properly recognize the emotions on the faces of others and their limited capacity to empathize with the emotional states of others (32).

According to our results, there was a direct and significant relationship between emotional irritability and current cravings and consumption thoughts. The association between aggressive behaviors, aggression, and emotional irritability with substance abuse has been repeatedly demonstrated. Accordingly, the component of emotional irritability and aggression is strongly associated with heavy alcohol and drug use (33). Based on the adaptive position-trait response model in the discussion of substance abuse, a person who is irritable and has negative emotions uses drugs as a way to relieve unpleasant states (34).

Negative emotion regulation strategies such as inability to control impulse, lack of emotional awareness, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, lack of emotional clarity, repression, and avoidance are significantly associated with increased consumption thoughts and fantasies (35). According to Koole (36), addicted people try to regulate their emotions with the aim of increasing pleasure and reducing suffering. Therefore, these patients regulate their emotions by using drugs, trying to prevent suffering and fatigue caused by their negative emotions. In the emotional model of user craving and bowling (2014), craving is known as a state of excitement that regulates it as a type of emotion regulation that directly reduces substance use.

Another finding of the study is that there is a significant relationship between negative and positive emotions and consumption thoughts. Positive and negative emotions are often referred to as predictors, consequences, and a definite feature of craving (37). Negative emotion develops in the form of feelings of guilt and anxiety in people who are actively trying to control drug use (38). Positive emotion can also enhance drug-related images and be more effective with strong and negative hybrid effects (39). In fact, positive emotion, as a motivational-appetite stimulant, shares the common feature of the pleasurable elements of drug use, while at the same time leading to the activation or sensitivity of the brain’s reward nervous systems.

Finally, impairment of emotional processing is a predictor of current cravings and thoughts and fantasies of consumption in opioid-dependent patients. Defects in emotion processing are actually related to the extent and severity of dependence as defects in reassessment (12).

5.1. Conclusions

The present study showed that, in general, there is a significant relationship between emotional components and craving. Therefore, the findings emphasize that the study of emotional factors related to the tendency of people to addiction is of particular importance, and in the field of substance abuse prevention, educating people to improve emotion regulation can be helpful. Hence, simultaneous attention to biological, psychological, and social dimensions can be very effective in preventing substance abuse.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    World Health Organization. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2018. Available from: https://www.who.int/standards/classifications/classification-of-diseases.

  • 2.

    Alon G, Yungher DA, Shulman LM, Rogers MW. Safety and immediate effect of noninvasive transcranial pulsed current stimulation on gait and balance in Parkinson disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2012;26(9):1089-95. [PubMed ID: 22581566]. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968312448233.

  • 3.

    Kassel JD. Substance abuse and emotion. Washington, USA: American Psychological Association; 2010. https://doi.org/10.1037/12067-000.

  • 4.

    Koob GF. The dark side of emotion: the addiction perspective. Eur J Pharmacol. 2015;753:73-87. [PubMed ID: 25583178]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4380644]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.11.044.

  • 5.

    van Steenbergen H, Eikemo M, Leknes S. The role of the opioid system in decision making and cognitive control: A review. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2019;19(3):435-58. [PubMed ID: 30963411]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6599188]. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00710-6.

  • 6.

    Quirk SW. Emotion concepts in models of substance abuse. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2001;20(1):95-104. https://doi.org/10.1080/09595230125185.

  • 7.

    Dorison CA, Wang K, Rees VW, Kawachi I, Ericson KMM, Lerner JS. Sadness, but not all negative emotions, heightens addictive substance use. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2020;117(2):943-9. [PubMed ID: 31888990]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6969515]. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1909888116.

  • 8.

    Estevez A, Jauregui P, Sanchez-Marcos I, Lopez-Gonzalez H, Griffiths MD. Attachment and emotion regulation in substance addictions and behavioral addictions. J Behav Addict. 2017;6(4):534-44. [PubMed ID: 29280395]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6034944]. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.086.

  • 9.

    Baharvand P, Malekshahi F. Relationship between anger and drug addiction potential as factors affecting the health of medical students. J Educ Health Promot. 2019;8:157. [PubMed ID: 31544122]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6745884]. https://doi.org/10.4103/jehp.jehp_145_19.

  • 10.

    Zarshenas L, Baneshi M, Sharif F, Moghimi Sarani E. Anger management in substance abuse based on cognitive behavioral therapy: An interventional study. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):375. [PubMed ID: 29169338]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5701421]. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1511-z.

  • 11.

    Bor M, Zareban I, Bakhshani N, Bakhshani S. Emotion regulation difficulties in drug abusers. Ann Trop Med Public Health. 2017;10(6):1724. https://doi.org/10.4103/atmph.atmph_617_17.

  • 12.

    Murphy A, Taylor E, Elliott R. The detrimental effects of emotional process dysregulation on decision-making in substance dependence. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012;6:101. [PubMed ID: 23162443]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3491319]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2012.00101.

  • 13.

    Franken IH, Hendriksa VM, van den Brink W. Initial validation of two opiate craving questionnaires the obsessive compulsive drug use scale and the desires for drug questionnaire. Addict Behav. 2002;27(5):675-85. [PubMed ID: 12201376]. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00201-5.

  • 14.

    Moosavi S, Kafi SM, Haghiri M, Ofoghi N, Atashkar SR, Abolghasemi S. Comparison of efficiency of cognitive therapy and logo therapy on the depression rate of aged men. Int J Psychol Couns. 2012;4(11):143-9.

  • 15.

    Fadardi JS, Ziaee SS, Barerfan Z. The Persian Post-Detoxification Craving and Temptation Scale. Mashhad. 2008;Unpublished.

  • 16.

    Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional Assessment of Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation: Development, Factor Structure, and Initial Validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26(1):41-54. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:joba.0000007455.08539.94.

  • 17.

    Azizi AR, Mirzaei A, Shams J. [Correlation between distress tolerance and emotional regulation with students smoking dependence]. Hakim Research Journal. 2010;13(1):11-8. Persian.

  • 18.

    Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348-62. [PubMed ID: 12916575]. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.

  • 19.

    Hasani J. [The psychometric properties of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ)]. J Clin Psychol. 2010;2(3):73-84. Persian.

  • 20.

    Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063-70. [PubMed ID: 3397865]. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063.

  • 21.

    Baker R, Thomas S, Thomas PW, Gower P, Santonastaso M, Whittlesea A. The Emotional Processing Scale: scale refinement and abridgement (EPS-25). J Psychosom Res. 2010;68(1):83-8. [PubMed ID: 20004304]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.07.007.

  • 22.

    Taklavi S, Zare R. [Role of Self-Conscious Affect and Theory of Mind in the Prediction of Justify Extramarital Relationships in Married Individuals]. Mod Psychol Stud. 2018;13(51):49-62. Persian.

  • 23.

    Gignac GE. Genos Emotional Intelligence Inventory. 2nd ed. Sydney, Waterloo; 2010.

  • 24.

    Braithwaite VA. The Scale of Emotional Arousability: bridging the gap between the neuroticism construct and its measurement. Psychol Med. 1987;17(1):217-25. [PubMed ID: 3575574]. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700013106.

  • 25.

    Skowron EA, Schmitt TA. Assessing interpersonal fusion: Reliability and validity of a new DSI fusion with others subscale. J Marital Fam Ther. 2003;29(2):209-22. [PubMed ID: 12728779]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.2003.tb01201.x.

  • 26.

    Rafi L. [Comparison of the level of differentiation of children of conflicting families with children of non-conflicting families [master's thesis]]. Tehran, Iran: University of Tehran; 2007.

  • 27.

    Farnam A, Mahmoodzehi S. [The Relationship of Negative Emotions with Craving and Relapse Rate in Substance Dependent Subjects with Relapse: Mediating Role of Social Support]. Social Psychology Research. 2019;33:1-18. Persian.

  • 28.

    Bassak Nejad S, Aarefi N, Arshadi N. Testing a model of Emotional Eating, Emotional Dysregulation, Self-Esteem, and Impulsivity in Food Addiction among students. Quarterly Journal of Health Psychology. 2018;7(26):132-45.

  • 29.

    Kashanki H, Rostami R, Dehghani Arai F. [Excitement and Addiction: A Review Study]. Social Health and Addiction Quarterly. 2018;5(18). Persian.

  • 30.

    Deng LY, Liu L, Xia CC, Lan J, Zhang JT, Fang XY. Craving Behavior Intervention in Ameliorating College Students' Internet Game Disorder: A Longitudinal Study. Front Psychol. 2017;8:526. [PubMed ID: 28443046]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5385373]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00526.

  • 31.

    Berking M, Margraf M, Ebert D, Wupperman P, Hofmann SG, Junghanns K. Deficits in emotion-regulation skills predict alcohol use during and after cognitive-behavioral therapy for alcohol dependence. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011;79(3):307-18. [PubMed ID: 21534653]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3109184]. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023421.

  • 32.

    Russell JA. Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychol Bull. 1991;110(3):426-50. [PubMed ID: 1758918]. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.426.

  • 33.

    Giancola PR. Irritability, acute alcohol consumption and aggressive behavior in men and women. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;68(3):263-74. [PubMed ID: 12393221]. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00221-1.

  • 34.

    Gilbert DG. The Situation × Trait Adaptive Response (STAR) Model of Drug Use, Effects, and Craving. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 1997;12(Suppl 2):S89-S102. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1099-1077(199706)12:2.

  • 35.

    Macklem GL. Practitioner's guide to emotion regulation in school-aged children. Berlin, Germany: Springer Science & Business Media; 2007.

  • 36.

    Koole SL. The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cogn Emot. 2009;23(1):4-41. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802619031.

  • 37.

    Schlauch RC, Gwynn-Shapiro D, Stasiewicz PR, Molnar DS, Lang AR. Affect and craving: Positive and negative affect are differentially associated with approach and avoidance inclinations. Addict Behav. 2013;38(4):1970-9. [PubMed ID: 23380493]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3578130]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.12.003.

  • 38.

    Kavanagh DJ, Andrade J, May J. Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychol Rev. 2005;112(2):446-67. [PubMed ID: 15783293]. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.446.

  • 39.

    Mason BJ, Light JM, Escher T, Drobes DJ. Effect of positive and negative affective stimuli and beverage cues on measures of craving in non treatment-seeking alcoholics. Psychopharmacology. 2008;200(1):141-50. [PubMed ID: 18604601]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC2758314]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1192-x.