Comparing the Executive Functions of Mothers with and Without Autistic Children

authors:

avatar Nasser Moradi 1 , * , avatar Nour-Mohammad Bakhshani ORCID 2 , avatar Jafar Sarani Yaztappeh ORCID 3

Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Medicine, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, Iran
Children and Adolescents Health Research Center, Resistant Tuberculosis Institute, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan, Iran
Ph.D. Student in Clinical Psychology, Department of Clinical Psychology, Medicine Faculty, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran

how to cite: Moradi N, Bakhshani N, Sarani Yaztappeh J. Comparing the Executive Functions of Mothers with and Without Autistic Children. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. 2021;15(1):e104092. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs.104092.

Abstract

Background:

Measuring the executive functioning in parents of children with autism is a way to specify the role of different aspects of executive functioning in the etiology of autism.

Objectives:

This study aimed to compare the executive functioning in mothers of children with and without autism.

Methods:

Following a case-control design, this study was conducted on 60 people (mothers of children with (n = 30) and without autism (n = 30) referred to the welfare rehabilitation centers of Zahedan, South East of Iran, in 2017 (March to September). Participants were selected by convenience sampling techniques. Participants were assessed using the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory, cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (Garnefski block design), digit span, and picture arrangement subscales of Wechsler Adult Intelligence (WAIS-IV) scale.

Results:

Data analysis showed no significant difference between the executive functioning in mothers of children with and without autism (P > 0.05), except for other-blame that belongs to the emotional regulation subscale (from the executive function factors) (P = 0.048).

Conclusions:

This study demonstrated that the parents of children with autism do not necessarily have executive functioning deficits. Therefore, it is necessary to consider other psychological and neuropsychological aspects in future studies.

1. Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by persistent deficiencies in social communication, behavioral patterns, and repetitive and limited interests or activities (1). The global prevalence of autism has increased significantly in recent years (2, 3). A comprehensive review estimated a global prevalence of 17 per 10,000 carriers (2). While the main symptom of autism is social failure (4), some researchers believe that failure in executive functions constitutes the main disorder of those who suffer from autism (5, 6). Executive functions are a set of processes that enable individuals to consciously control their behavior and thoughts with respect to their future goals (7). As crucial nerve structures, these functions are generally referred to as the high psychological processes involved in controlling and regulating the cognition, thinking, goal-oriented behavior, and anticipation functions (8, 9). Executive functions include impulse control, self-regulation, initiation, working memory, mental flexibility, ability to deal with new things, social thinking, sustained attention, etc. (10, 11).

Emotion regulation (ER) is a construct that may provide explanatory power for understanding the observed emotional and behavioral problems in ASD (12). Impaired ER has been associated with several disorders, including anxiety, mood disorders, borderline personality disorder, and ASD (13, 14). ER is conceptualized as an executive function that plays a role in regulating and inhibiting behaviors. While this executive function has a neurobiological basis, parents play a central role in the development of ER via soothing, organizing, and refocusing. Indeed, they enhance the development of more independent regulatory strategies in typically developing children (15).

Previous findings suggested that adolescents who rely on reappraisal may have more cognitive resources to help them remain attentive and well-regulated in their daily lives. On the other hand, if better executive functions facilitate the use of reappraisal, adolescents’ ability to regulate their emotions could potentially be enhanced via supports for executive functions (16). Parenting is a cognitive, emotional, and behavioral endeavor; Shaffer and Obradovic (17) reported that direct assessment of parent inhibitory control was positively associated with sensitive/responsive behaviors, whereas parent self-reported difficulties in using emotion regulation strategies were associated with lower levels of positive and collaborative dyadic behaviors. In addition, Hinnant et al. (18) indicated that children from more co-operative dyads, who possessed higher executive function skills, had higher moral reasoning scores than other children. Besides, they reported that children lower in both emotion regulation and executive function had lower moral reasoning scores than other children (18).

Many children with autism suffer from impaired executive function (19, 20). Measuring the executive functioning in parents of children with autism is a way to indicate aspects of executive functions, which may play an important role in the development of this disorder (21). Bolte and Poustka (22) investigated and compared executive functioning in parents of children with autism and schizophrenia and mental retardation and parents of healthy children. They found no statistically significant differences in the executive functioning between parents of children with autism and other parents (22). Another study, which compared the executive functioning of parents and siblings of autistic children and those with normal children, mentioned some weaknesses in some executive functioning abilities (e.g., planning) in parents and siblings of autistic children (23). Also, Moazzen et al. (21) investigated executive functioning of first-degree relatives of autistic patients and reported difficulties in inhibition and cognitive flexibility functions.

2. Objectives

Accordingly, the current study aimed to compare the executive functioning in mothers of children with and without autism.

3. Methods

The current descriptive and case-control study is approved by the ethical committee of Zahedan University of Medical Sciences. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants (code no.: 7889). The study population included all parents with autistic children referred to welfare rehabilitation centers of Zahedan, South East of Iran, in 2017 (March to September). The study sample consisted of 60 mothers who were selected by convenience sampling technique (30 mothers with autistic children and 30 mothers without autistic children). Those with a history of psychiatric disorders, head injury, or brain tumor were excluded from the study. Furthermore, participants were matched in terms of age, education, and culture of mothers, the order of birth, gender, and age of children. Demographic characteristics of mothers of children with and without autism are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of Mothers of Children with and Without Autism

VariableNo. (%)
Age, y
Mothers
25 - 3028 (46)
31 - 3516 (26)
36 - 404 (6)
41 - 458 (13)
46 - 504 (6)
Children
2 - 524 (40)
6 - 1032 (53)
11 - 154 (6)
Education
Below high school diploma2 (3)
High school diploma4 (6)
Undergraduate44 (73)
Graduate10 (16)
Birth order of children
First-born32 (53)
Second-born20 (33)
Third-born8 (13)
Child’s gender
Boy48 (80)
Girl12 (20)

3.1. Instruments

3.1.1. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV)

This test includes some subscales such as digit span, block design, and picture arrangement, which are explained below.

3.1.1.1. Digit Span

In this test, participants hear a series of numbers, which starts from three-digit numbers and ends with nine-digit numbers and should repeat them. Two-digit sequences with different numbers are given to the participants. The total score is equal to the number of correct repetitions. The test ends after one mistake. The other form of this test involves repeating the list of numbers in reverse order (24). In the present study, this test was used to measure working memory.

3.1.1.2. Block Design

This test features 9 colored cubes (white, red, and red-white). The participant must arrange the cubes at the specified time as required by Wechsler test. The sooner s/he makes the related arrangements, the greater will be the performance. This test is used to measure organization and problem-solving items (25).

3.1.1.3. Picture Arrangement

It contains 8 components arranged as a series of illustrated cards that tell a story when put in a certain order. The picture arrangement test intends to examine the social relationships (26).

These three tests are the subscale of the Revised Wechsler Adult Intelligence scale. The validity of these three subscales is reported between 0.76 and 0.97 (27).

3.2. Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ)

This 36-item questionnaire intends to measure the subscale of cognitive-emotional regulation: self-blame, other-blame, rumination, catastrophizing, putting into perspective, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, acceptance, and refocus on planning. In other words, it is designed to evaluate the cognitive strategies that each person uses after experiencing threatening events or life stresses (28). In Iran, reported Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the subscales of this questionnaire range from 0.76 to 0.92; and the values reported for Kendal’s coefficient range from 0.81 to 0.92 (29).

The scoring method according to the Likert scale was as follows: never (1), rarely (2), sometimes (3), often (4), and always (5). The minimum score was 36, and the maximum score was 180. Based on the calculated scores, the cognitive emotion regulation rate was poor, medium, and strong (36 - 72, 72 - 108, and > 108, respectively).

3.3. Cognitive Flexibility Inventory (CFI) (Denis and Vander Wal)

The CFI contains two subscales and intends to measure cognitive flexibility. The alternatives and control subscale measures three aspects of cognitive flexibility: (A) the tendency to perceive difficult situations as controllable; (B) the ability to perceive multiple alternative explanations for life occurrences and human behavior; and (C) the ability to generate multiple alternative solutions to difficult situations. Each item is scored on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = strong disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = slightly disagree, 4 = no idea, 5 = agree a bit, 6 = agree, and 7 = strong agree). The total score ranges from 20 to 140. The concurrent validity of the CFI was -0.39 as obtained by Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), and its convergent validity was 0.75 using the Cognitive Flexibility scale (CFS) proposed by Martin and Rubin (30). In Iran, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the subscales and the whole questionnaire are reported at 0.90 and 0.71, respectively (27).

3.4. Data Collection

The participants were tested individually in a quiet room. Each day two participants were examined. First, the respondents answered CFI and CERQ. Then, the respondents had a break for 5 to 15 minutes. After the break, the Wechsler subscales, including block design, digit span test, and picture arrangement, were administered.

3.5. Statistical Analysis

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) test was used to perform intra-group and intergroup comparisons of executive functions. Data were analyzed using SPSS version 16. Statistical significance was considered when P-value < 0.05.

4. Results

Mean executive functioning scores (organization, problem-solving, cognitive flexibility, emotional adjustment, working memory, and Social relations) of mothers of children with and without autism are shown in Table 2. The results showed no significant difference between the executive functioning scores of parents of children with and without autism, as shown in Table 3 (P > 0.05). However, the evaluation of the organization and problem-solving components between the two groups showed marginally insignificant differences (P = 0.083). A significant difference was found in one of the factors of emotional regulation (i.e. other-blame), in which parents without autistic children had a higher score (P = 0.048).

Table 2.

Mean and Standard Deviation of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Variables for Both Groups (Mothers with Autistic Children (Case Group) and Mothers Without Autistic Children (Control Group))

Cognitive Emotion Regulation VariablesMean± SD
Organization
Case23.37 ± 7.867
Control26.80 ± 7.203
Problem-solving
Case23.37 ± 7.867
Control26.80 ± 7.203
Cognitive flexibility
Case90.60 ± 14.642
Control93.73 ± 11.659
Positive emotional regulation
Case46.37 ± 10.193
Control47.30 ± 8.603
Negative emotional regulation
Case61.63 ± 12.732
Control63.13 ± 11.181
Working memory
Case14.17 ± 3.705
Control15.63 ± 3.605
Social relations
Case12.77 ± 4.546
Control13.40 ± 5.117
Table 3.

Results of ANOVA for Cognitive Emotion Regulation Variables Between Groups, Intergroup, and Total Participants

Cognitive Emotion Regulation VariablesSSdfMSFSig
Organization3.1080.083
Between groups176.8171176.817
Intergroup3299.7675856.893
Total3476.58359
Problem-solving3.1080.083
Between groups176.8171176.817
Intergroup3299.7675856.893
Total3476.58359
Cognitive flexibility0.8410.363
Between groups147.2671147.267
Intragroup10159.06758175.156
Total10306.33359
Working memory2.4140.126
Between groups32.267132.267
Intragroup775.1335813.364
Total807.40059
Social relations0.2570.614
Between groups6.01716.017
Intragroup1358.5675823.424
Total1364.58359
Positive emotional regulation0.1470.703
Between groups13.067113.067
Intragroup5159.2675888.953
Total5172.33359
Negative emotional regulation0.2350.630
Between groups33.750133.750
Intragroup8326.43358143.559
Total8360.18359
Self-blame0.4320.514
Between groups6.01716.017
Intragroup807.63358
Total813.6505913.925
Acceptance0.1560.695
Between groups1.66711.667
Intragroup620.7335810.702
Total622.40059
Rumination0.1840.669
Between groups2.01712.017
Intragroup634.5675810.941
Total636.58359
Positive refocus0.9310.339
Between groups11.267111.267
Intragroup701.6675812.98
Total712.93359
Planning refocus0.0050.947
Between groups.6710.67
Intragroup857.86758
Total857.9335914.791
Positive reappraisal0.6410.427
Between groups9.60019.600
Intragroup868.73358
Total878.3335914.978
Putting into perspective0.0830.774
Between groups1.06711.067
Intragroup744.9335812.844
Total746.00059
Catastrophizing1.6650.202
Between groups20.417120.417
Intragroup711.2335812.263
Total731.65059
Other-blame4.0930.048
Between groups64.067164.07
Intragroup907.8675815.653
Total971.93359

5. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to compare the executive functions (e.g. organization, problem-solving, cognitive flexibility, working memory, and social relations) in parents of children with and without autism. According to the findings, there was no significant difference between mothers of children with and without autism concerning executive functions, except for other-blame.

Numerous studies reported that families with autistic disorder experience more stress than parents with typically developing children (31). It has been shown that parents with poor emotion regulation tend to perceive parenting responsibilities as more stressful than those with better emotion regulation (32). This might be particularly true for parents of children with ASD as the stress associated with parenting these children is already overwhelming. In a study on parents of children with ASD, Ekas et al. (33) reported that mothers’ and fathers’ use of emotional support from their partners could highly predict their relationship satisfaction. Cognitive emotion regulation is useful when someone is confronted with unpleasant and stressful events. It is believed that the use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies such as rumination, thought suppression, reevaluation, and problem-solving may be an important diagnostic criterion in different forms of psychopathology (34). Salimi et al. (35) conducted a study to investigate the effectiveness of group-based acceptance and commitment therapy on cognitive emotion regulation strategies in mothers of children with autism. They asked the respondents to fill the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire both before and after the intervention. Their results showed that group-based acceptance and commitment therapy had a significant effect on positive/planning strategy refocusing, positive reappraisal, self-blaming, and blaming others, considering a situation as disastrous, reception (35).

Kim et al. (36) reported an intergenerational association concerning the risk of ASD in executive function between mothers and children. In another study, Chico et al. (37) reported a significant correlation between the executive function of mothers and their children. Moreover, according to the literature, there is a gender-specific difference in the parent‐reported executive functioning and adaptive behavior in children and young adults with ASD (38). Furthermore, some studies suggested that parents of autistic children might have a range of autism problems (39, 40).

Moazzen et al. (21) found that the relatives of patients with autism had poorer performance in terms of cognitive flexibility and inhibition than the control group. Wong et al. (23) demonstrated that the relatives of patients with autism had poorer cognitive flexibility and performance than the control group, while there was no significant difference between the two groups concerning inhibition. In the same vein, Hughes et al. (41) also investigated planning, cognitive flexibility, and working memory in the parents and siblings of patients with autism and then compared the results with the control group as well as parents and siblings of people who had other developmental disorders. They concluded that relatives of autistic people had poorer planning and cognitive flexibility as compared with the two groups; however, they mentioned no difference between the three groups concerning the spatial working memory and the capacity of working memory (41). Our results are not consistent with the mentioned findings.

This discrepancy can be attributed to the differences in the methodology adopted by each study. In the present study, Wechsler test subscales, CFI, and CERQ were used to measure executive functions, while the other studies had employed Wisconsin and Stroop tests. Also, the above-mentioned studies have been conducted on first-degree relatives of autistic patients, including parents and siblings, but this study was carried out on mothers with autistic children.

Given the considerable effects of cultural differences on many aspects of human development and personality, the findings of the present study are not an exception, so that one may explain the higher level of ‘other-blame’ in parents without autistic children by reference to such variations (42). As mentioned before, this study was performed in the city of Zahedan, whose inhabitants generally have more solid spiritual beliefs and interpret events and life experiences from a spiritual perspective. Hence, it can be speculated that people who grow in this cultural community are less likely to accuse and criticize others for various problems. Because of the extreme and unusual nature of the incident or problem in question, they are less likely to hold others responsible, instead relate it to Providence.

It is necessary to mention some limitations of this study, including the small sample size. Besides, the current study was conducted in a homogenous population of Zahedan, in which the two groups (mothers with and without autistic children) were finely matched regarding the age, education, and culture of mothers, as well as the order of birth, gender, and age of children. Moreover, we evaluated six items of executive functions in the present study, while previous studies mostly used two or three items. Against some previous studies, we replaced the Denis and Vander Wal questionnaires instead of the Wisconsin card test. Because before the implementation of the study, we carried out a Wisconsin test on a group of students (medical, nursing, environmental health students, and staff of the Zahedan University of Medical Sciences) as well as uneducated people. We found that due to the complexity of test execution, the majority of participants were reluctant to take the test. Therefore in the current study, Denis and Vander Wal questionnaires were used in order to, firstly, avoid tiredness of participants tired and, secondly, interfering with the results of the test.

This study demonstrated that the parents (mothers) of children with autism did not have any impairment in their executive functioning abilities, and it is likely that parents who do not have an autistic child might experience such impairments because parents of children with autism had once children who did not have this disorder. As we could not find any similar study to compare the executive functions addressed in the present study (e.g. as organization, problem-solving, emotional regulation, and social relations), it is recommended to perform further studies in this field.

Future studies, with a larger sample size, are needed to extend our knowledge. Also, considering both parents will provide considerable biological-psychological-cultural information for assessing variations among study participants.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Murphy CM, Wilson CE, Robertson DM, Ecker C, Daly EM, Hammond N, et al. Autism spectrum disorder in adults: diagnosis, management, and health services development. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2016;12:1669-86. [PubMed ID: 27462160]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4940003]. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S65455.

  • 2.

    Elsabbagh M, Divan G, Koh YJ, Kim YS, Kauchali S, Marcin C, et al. Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Res. 2012;5(3):160-79. [PubMed ID: 22495912]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3763210]. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.239.

  • 3.

    Hansen SN, Schendel DE, Parner ET. Explaining the increase in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: the proportion attributable to changes in reporting practices. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(1):56-62. [PubMed ID: 25365033]. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.1893.

  • 4.

    Frye RE. Social Skills Deficits in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Potential Biological Origins and Progress in Developing Therapeutic Agents. CNS Drugs. 2018;32(8):713-34. [PubMed ID: 30105528]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6105175]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40263-018-0556-y.

  • 5.

    Johnston K, Murray K, Spain D, Walker I, Russell A. Executive Function: Cognition and Behaviour in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(10):4181-92. [PubMed ID: 31281952]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6751156]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04133-7.

  • 6.

    Craig F, Margari F, Legrottaglie AR, Palumbi R, de Giambattista C, Margari L. A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2016;12:1191-202. [PubMed ID: 27274255]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4869784]. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S104620.

  • 7.

    Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-68. [PubMed ID: 23020641]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4084861]. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

  • 8.

    Miyake A, Emerson MJ, Friedman NP. Assessment of executive functions in clinical settings: problems and recommendations. Semin Speech Lang. 2000;21(2):169-83. [PubMed ID: 10879548]. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2000-7563.

  • 9.

    Garcia-Madruga JA, Gomez-Veiga I, Vila JO. Executive Functions and the Improvement of Thinking Abilities: The Intervention in Reading Comprehension. Front Psychol. 2016;7:58. [PubMed ID: 26869961]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4740460]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00058.

  • 10.

    Gokcen E, Frederickson N, Petrides KV. Theory of Mind and Executive Control Deficits in Typically Developing Adults and Adolescents with High Levels of Autism Traits. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(6):2072-87. [PubMed ID: 26886468]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4860196]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2735-3.

  • 11.

    Davids RC, Groen Y, Berg IJ, Tucha OM, van Balkom ID. Executive Functions in Older Adults With Autism Spectrum Disorder: Objective Performance and Subjective Complaints. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(9):2859-73. [PubMed ID: 27278313]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2831-4.

  • 12.

    Mazefsky CA, Herrington J, Siegel M, Scarpa A, Maddox BB, Scahill L, et al. The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(7):679-88. [PubMed ID: 23800481]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3719386]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006.

  • 13.

    Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):217-37. [PubMed ID: 20015584]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.

  • 14.

    Cai RY, Richdale AL, Uljarevic M, Dissanayake C, Samson AC. Emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder: Where we are and where we need to go. Autism Res. 2018;11(7):962-78. [PubMed ID: 29979494]. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1968.

  • 15.

    Crowell JA, Keluskar J, Gorecki A. Parenting behavior and the development of children with autism spectrum disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2019;90:21-9. [PubMed ID: 30658339]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.11.007.

  • 16.

    Lantrip C, Isquith PK, Koven NS, Welsh K, Roth RM. Executive Function and Emotion Regulation Strategy Use in Adolescents. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2016;5(1):50-5. [PubMed ID: 25650638]. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2014.960567.

  • 17.

    Shaffer A, Obradovic J. Unique contributions of emotion regulation and executive functions in predicting the quality of parent-child interaction behaviors. J Fam Psychol. 2017;31(2):150-9. [PubMed ID: 27929314]. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000269.

  • 18.

    Hinnant JB, Nelson JA, O'Brien M, Keane SP, Calkins SD. The interactive roles of parenting, emotion regulation and executive functioning in moral reasoning during middle childhood. Cogn Emot. 2013;27(8):1460-8. [PubMed ID: 23650955]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3751970]. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.789792.

  • 19.

    Chan AS, Cheung MC, Han YM, Sze SL, Leung WW, Man HS, et al. Executive function deficits and neural discordance in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009;120(6):1107-15. [PubMed ID: 19442578]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2009.04.002.

  • 20.

    Endedijk H, Denessen E, Hendriks AW. Relationships between executive functioning and homework difficulties in students with and without autism spectrum disorder: An analysis of student- and parent-reports. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(6):765-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.016.

  • 21.

    Moazzen M, Yaghooti F, Saleh J. Executive Functions in Parents and Siblings of Children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorders. Q J Child Ment Health. 2015;2(2):85-91.

  • 22.

    Bolte S, Poustka F. The broader cognitive phenotype of autism in parents: how specific is the tendency for local processing and executive dysfunction? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):639-45. [PubMed ID: 16712641]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01603.x.

  • 23.

    Wong D, Maybery M, Bishop DV, Maley A, Hallmayer J. Profiles of executive function in parents and siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Genes Brain Behav. 2006;5(8):561-76. [PubMed ID: 17081261]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00199.x.

  • 24.

    Jones G, Macken B. Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition. 2015;144:1-13. [PubMed ID: 26209910]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009.

  • 25.

    Groth-Marnat G, Teal M. Block design as a measure of everyday spatial ability: a study of ecological validity. Percept Mot Skills. 2000;90(2):522-6. [PubMed ID: 10833749]. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2000.90.2.522.

  • 26.

    Kaufman AS, Flanagan DP, Alfonso VC, Mascolo JT. Test Review: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). J Psychoeducat Assess. 2016;24(3):278-95. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282906288389.

  • 27.

    Nejati V. Cognitive abilities questionnaire: Development and evaluation of psychometric properties. Adv Cogn Sci. 2013;15(2):11-9.

  • 28.

    Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. Manual for the use of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Leiderdorp, The Netherlands: DATEC; 2012. https://doi.org/10.1037/t03801-000.

  • 29.

    Hasani J, Miraghaie AM. The relationship between strategies for cognitive regulation of emotions and suicidal ideation. Contemp Psychol Biann J Iran Psychol Assoc. 2012;7(1):61-72.

  • 30.

    Dennis JP, Vander Wal JS. The Cognitive Flexibility Inventory: Instrument Development and Estimates of Reliability and Validity. Cogn Ther Res. 2009;34(3):241-53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-009-9276-4.

  • 31.

    Hu X, Han ZR, Bai L, Gao MM. The Mediating Role of Parenting Stress in the Relations Between Parental Emotion Regulation and Parenting Behaviors in Chinese Families of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Dyadic Analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(10):3983-98. [PubMed ID: 31197635]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6751273]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04103-z.

  • 32.

    Deater-Deckard K, Li M, Bell MA. Multifaceted emotion regulation, stress and affect in mothers of young children. Cogn Emot. 2016;30(3):444-57. [PubMed ID: 25759238]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4567550]. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1013087.

  • 33.

    Ekas NV, Timmons L, Pruitt M, Ghilain C, Alessandri M. The Power of Positivity: Predictors of Relationship Satisfaction for Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(7):1997-2007. [PubMed ID: 25601217]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2362-4.

  • 34.

    Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: a transdiagnostic examination. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(10):974-83. [PubMed ID: 20591413]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002.

  • 35.

    Salimi M, Mahdavi A, Yeghaneh SS, Abedin M, Hajhosseini M. The Effectiveness of Group Based Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) on Emotion Cognitive Regulation Strategies in Mothers of Children with Autism Spectrum. Maedica (Bucur). 2019;14(3):240-6. [PubMed ID: 31798739]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6861729]. https://doi.org/10.26574/maedica.2019.14.3.240.

  • 36.

    Kim MH, Shimomaeda L, Giuliano RJ, Skowron EA. Intergenerational associations in executive function between mothers and children in the context of risk. J Exp Child Psychol. 2017;164:1-15. [PubMed ID: 28759782]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5591781]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.002.

  • 37.

    Chico E, Gonzalez A, Ali N, Steiner M, Fleming AS. Executive function and mothering: challenges faced by teenage mothers. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56(5):1027-35. [PubMed ID: 24523069]. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21185.

  • 38.

    White EI, Wallace GL, Bascom J, Armour AC, Register-Brown K, Popal HS, et al. Sex differences in parent-reported executive functioning and adaptive behavior in children and young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2017;10(10):1653-62. [PubMed ID: 28568910]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5721669]. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1811.

  • 39.

    Lau WYP, Peterson CC, Attwood T, Garnett MS, Kelly AB. Parents on the autism continuum: Links with parenting efficacy. Res Autism Spectrum Disord. 2016;26:57-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.02.007.

  • 40.

    Krakovich TM, McGrew JH, Yu Y, Ruble LA. Stress in Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Exploration of Demands and Resources. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(6):2042-53. [PubMed ID: 26883644]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2728-2.

  • 41.

    Hughes C, Leboyer M, Bouvard M. Executive function in parents of children with autism. Psychol Med. 1997;27(1):209-20. [PubMed ID: 9122301]. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291796004308.

  • 42.

    De Leersnyder J, Boiger M, Mesquita B. Cultural regulation of emotion: individual, relational, and structural sources. Front Psychol. 2013;4:55. [PubMed ID: 23408753]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3569661]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00055.