Abstract
Background:
In recent years, particular emphasis has been placed on the role of mental imagery in psychopathologies.Objectives:
The current study aimed to investigate the psychometric properties of the Persian version of the Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS-P) in the general Iranian population.Methods:
The current psychometric study was carried out on 474 Iranian individuals in 2020 through convenience sampling. The SUIS, Ambiguous Scenarios Test relevant to Depressed Mood (AST-D), and Beck Depression Inventory-second edition (BDI-II) were filled out by the study subjects. Moreover, for the determination of divergent validity between the SUIS and BDI-II, the Pearson correlation was also used. Data analysis was performed by SPSS Software (version 24).Results:
The internal consistency of the total score of the SUIS was acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75). The significant positive relationship between the SUIS and AST-D (r = 0.34; P < 0.01) showed good convergent validity. No significant correlation was observed between the SUIS and BDI-II. Factor analysis using the principal component analysis with a varimax rotation introduced four factors. The four factors explained 0.55 of the total variance. Regarding construct validity, the inter-item correlation and item-total correlations of the SUIS-P were in an acceptable range.Conclusions:
Based on the results, the SUIS has excellent psychometric properties in the Iranian population and can be used in research and therapeutic interventions.Keywords
Iranian Population Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale Psychometric Properties
1. Background
Mental images play an important role in individuals’ lives. In everyday life, thoughts about past or future events often appear as mental images. Mental imagery is defined as the experience of seeing with the mind’s eye, hearing with the mind’s ear, and others (1). Mental imagery can be important for cognitive functions, such as learning, memory, and problem-solving (2). In addition, imagination might be essential in developing and maintaining psychiatric disorders (3, 4). Imagination can also contribute significantly to the development and recovery of emotional states (5-7). In studies based on mental imagery, a scale that can measure the participants’ differences in their mental imagination ability at the baseline stage will be helpful (7). Despite the importance of mental imagery in psychiatric disorders, its evaluation in the conceptualization and treatment of disorders has been neglected.
Evaluating the generation of clear and vivid mental imagery has been of interest to researchers for many years, and several scales have been developed in this regard, for example, the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ) (8) and Questionnaire upon Mental Imagery (QMI) (9). In previous studies, these measuring instruments were associated with cognitive functions, such as attention and memory performance, and with creativity and affective reactions (4, 8, 10, 11). However, the Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS) was developed by Kosslyn (12), which seems to measure the habitual use of mental imagery as a trait. Therefore, the SUIS assessed some aspects that have been missed in previous studies.
The SUIS is a 12-item self-report scale developed by Reisberg et al. (13) in 2003 to evaluate individual differences in imagination ability in daily life on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 “never” to 5 “always” (12). Higher values in the SUIS indicate a stronger tendency for mental imagery. This scale emphasizes only visual imagination, and other modalities, such as auditory imagination, are not assessed by the SUIS (4). The SUIS has high internal consistency and good convergent validity (13-15). The SUIS emphasizes the likelihood and frequency of imagination in an everyday situation. However, other tools of imagination ability, such as the QMI (9) and VVIQ (8), emphasize the vividness and quality of imagination.
In recent years, research into mental imagery has gained increasing importance, especially in clinical psychology, and the SUIS is also a frequently used instrument in this regard (16). Previous research has revealed (17) that mental imagery affects our emotional experience more than verbal processing. For this reason, theoretical models have recently been developed for the special importance of emotion-enhancing mental images in emotional disorders (18). In English-speaking countries, the SUIS is a widely used tool for the self-assessment of imagery propensity (4, 12). However, to date, no research has been conducted in Iran to investigate the psychometric properties of the SUIS, and there is no information on the test psychometric properties of this scale.
2. Objectives
This study aimed to translate the SUIS into Persian and determine its validity, reliability, and factor structure in the Iranian population.
3. Methods
The design of this study was a cross-sectional descriptive correlational analysis. The statistical sample comprised 474 individuals in 2020 using the convenience sampling method. The original English version of the scale was initially translated into Persian by a scientifically active psychologist. In the second stage, the items were translated into English by a bilingual translator with particular experience in psychology. The two English versions are very similar. No items were omitted or changed. The data were collected online through social media; accordingly, after distributing the announcement in cyberspace (e.g., Instagram, Telegram, and WhatsApp), the forms were sent to the participants.
The inclusion criteria included Iranian nationality, education of at least a diploma, age range of 18 - 65 years, and ability to read and write. The exclusion criteria included lack of cooperation, inability to complete the questionnaire for any reason, and lack of interest in continuing the study. An informed consent form was provided for the participants, and all participants gave their consent either in writing or verbally. Subsequently, the study subjects filled out the online study’s main questionnaire. Before responding to the questionnaire’s items, the subjects were asked to record demographic information.
3.1. Ethical Considerations and Informed Consent
The study was conducted after receiving an ethics code from the University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran (approval code: IR.USWR.REC.1400.102). The present study followed all the ethical standards listed in the Declaration of Helsinki.
3.2. Statistical Analysis
Descriptive and inferential statistical methods were utilized during this study. Descriptive methods were accustomed to calculating central indicators and dispersion. The exploratory multivariate analysis method was utilized to determine the questionnaire’s subscales. SPSS software (version 24) was used for statistical analyses. The demographic data were analyzed using mean (M), variance (standard deviation [SD]), and descriptive statistics before the inferential analysis. Additionally, the outlier’s data were checked via z-scores and box plots (values + 3 standard deviations from the mean indicating univariate outliers). A visual check showed that the information was distributed normally. Reliability, which relates to the consistency and stability of a measure, was assessed with the internal consistency of tests and retests (i.e., Cronbach’s α). The Ambiguous Scenarios Test relevant to Depressed Mood (AST-D) and Beck Depression Inventory- second edition (BDI-II) were used for convergent and divergent validity, respectively.
All participants voluntarily participated or did not participate in the study. Additionally, the examiners pledged that the individual’s data remain confidential and every participant will access the questionnaire results.
3.3. Measures
The Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale: The SUIS, as a 12-item questionnaire, was designed for the measurement of the spontaneous use of images in daily life (13). A 5-point scale is utilized for rating the level of the appropriateness of each item (from “never appropriate” to “entirely appropriate”). A complete score is usually computed by summing the scores of the 12 items, resulting in a complete score range of 12-60, in which higher scores show more use of imagining in everyday life. The SUIS’s internal consistency is reported within an acceptable range (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83) (4).
The Beck Depression Inventory- second edition: This scale is the revised version of the BDI (19), including 21 items. Each question’s four choices are scored on a four-level scale from 0 to 3. As a result, the questionnaire’s total score is within the range of 0 - 63. Additionally, the internal consistency of this inventory is claimed to be 0.91. (19). In Iran, the alpha coefficient was 0.91, the correlation coefficient between the two halves was 0.89, and the one-week retest coefficient was 0.94 (20).
The Ambiguous Scenarios Test relevant to Depressed Mood II (AST-D-II) (Long Version): The AST-D-II is a 30-item self-report test that asks the individuals to imagine each scenario (e.g., one’s performance and skills, past experiences, future situations) as vividly as possible. Then, it rates the level of the pleasantness of each situation on an 11-point Likert scale ranging from -5 (extremely unpleasant) to +5 (extremely pleasant). The AST-D-II was developed to evaluate the interpretation of depression-related biases (higher scores indicating a more positive bias). The internal consistency of the AST-D-II in Rohrbacher’s study (21) was within an acceptable range (a = 0.87). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) showed that the three-factor models fit with the AST-D-II. In addition to convergent validity, the AST-D-II has a significant correlation with depression scores (21).
4. Results
The statistical sample of the current study was 474 subjects. The age range of the participants was within 18-58 years (M=30.33, SD=8.49). The participants included 270 (77.1%) and 70 (22.9%) female and male subjects, respectively. In addition, 164 (35.5%), 225 (47.4%), and 85 (17.01%) participants were married, single, and divorced, respectively. The SUIS response rate was around 78%. The scores of the SUIS were normally distributed, with no evidence of univariate or multivariate outliers. The distribution of the total values showed a skewness of -0.44 (SE = 0.15) and a kurtosis of 0.35 (SE = 0.29) within the normal range.
Analyses’ findings reported the relationship between the SUIS, BDI-II, and AST-D to determine convergent and discriminant validity. The SUIS correlated significantly with the AST-D vividness subscale (r = 0.34, P < 0.01) and AST-D pleasantness subscale (r = 0.21, P < 0.05). In addition, the SUIS did not significantly correlate with the BDI-II (r = -0.08, P > 0.05). The independent t-test showed no significant statistical differences between the SUIS and its subscale with the AST-D-II and BDI-II (Table 1). The mean total value of the 12 items was 44.0 (SD = 8.55). Female subjects (Mean = 42.87, SD = 7.06) achieved non-significantly higher total values than male subjects (Mean = 41.05, SD = 8.42, t [214] = 1.42, P > 0.05).
Correlation of Ambiguous Scenarios Test Relevant to Depressed Mood II and Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition with Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale a
Variable (Scales) | Mean ± SD | Correlation with SUIS |
---|---|---|
AST-D-II (vividness subscale) | 120.70 ± 20.68 | 0.34** |
AST-D-II (pleasantness subscale) | 83.36 ± 36.51 | 0.21* |
BDI-II | 10.68 ± 9.0 | 0.08 |
SUIS | 42.64 ± 7.32 | -- |
The internal consistency of the 12 items can be rated as acceptable with a Cronbach’s alpha value of 0.75. According to Table 2, the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the SUIS was within the range of 0.70 - 0.75.
Item Analysis
Scale Mean If Item Deleted | Scale Variance If Item Deleted | Corrected Item-Total Correlation | Squared Multiple Correlations | Cronbach’s Alpha If Item Deleted | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
SUIS 1 | 38.3 | 49.966 | 0.333 | 0.150 | 0.733 |
SUIS 2 | 38.9 | 46.337 | 0.442 | 0.231 | 0.719 |
SUIS 3 | 37.8 | 49.278 | 0.408 | 0.230 | 0.724 |
SUIS 4 | 38.0 | 51.527 | 0.219 | 0.080 | 0.747 |
SUIS 5 | 37.7 | 51.604 | 0.322 | 0.123 | 0.734 |
SUIS 6 | 39.8 | 53.629 | 0.109 | 0.055 | 0.758 |
SUIS 7 | 38.1 | 49.371 | 0.335 | 0.160 | 0.733 |
SUIS 8 | 37.9 | 48.952 | 0.427 | 0.312 | 0.722 |
SUIS 9 | 37.9 | 48.628 | 0.443 | 0.296 | 0.720 |
SUIS 10 | 38.5 | 45.888 | 0.535 | 0.528 | 0.706 |
SUIS 11 | 38.6 | 45.457 | 0.544 | 0.552 | 0.705 |
SUIS 12 | 39.2 | 47.757 | 0.426 | 0.269 | 0.721 |
The item difficulty was calculated using SPSS software (version 24). The difficulty of the individual items varied within the range of 0.25 - 0.78. Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics for any subscale and the total scale. Although item 6 had the lowest correlation with the total score, it was still kept in the final version of the Persian version of the Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS-P). According to the item-total correlation analysis presented in Table 3, all of the 12 items correlated significantly with each other and the total score of the SUIS-P (in the domain of 0.25 to 0.68, P < 0.01).
Inter-item and Item-Total Correlations of Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale a
SUIS1 | SUIS2 | SUIS3 | SUIS4 | SUIS5 | SUIS6 | SUIS7 | SUIS8 | SUIS9 | SUIS 10 | SUIS 11 | SUIS 12 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SUIS 1 | 1 | 0.25** | 0.20** | 0.12** | 0.11* | 0.06 | 0.15** | 0.21** | 0.29** | 0.16** | 0.13* | 0.19** |
SUIS 2 | 1 | 0.24** | 0.09 | 0.20** | 0.11* | 0.20** | 0.16** | 0.22** | 0.30** | 0.30** | 0.36** | |
SUIS 3 | 1 | 0.19** | 0.18** | 0.03 | 0.16** | 0.40** | 0.28** | 0.21** | 0.25** | 0.15** | ||
SUIS 4 | 1 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.11* | 0.21** | 0.15** | 0.13* | 0.07 | 0.04 | |||
SUIS 5 | 1 | -0.01 | 0.20** | 0.16** | 0.20** | 0.23** | 0.23** | 0.20** | ||||
SUIS 6 | 1 | -0.03 | 0.02 | -0.05 | 0.13** | 0.14** | 0.12* | |||||
SUIS 7 | 1 | 0.28** | 0.28** | 0.18** | 0.16** | 0.21** | ||||||
SUIS 8 | 1 | 0.41** | 0.18** | 0.26** | 0.12* | |||||||
SUIS 9 | 1 | 0.30** | 0.27** | 0.13* | ||||||||
SUIS 10 | 1 | 0.70** | 0.36** | |||||||||
SUIS 11 | 1 | 0.42** | ||||||||||
SUIS 12 | 1 |
With regard to the reduction of the sample size due to missing data that results in problems in exploratory factor analysis (EFA), the missing data replacement method was employed with the variable mean for missing data management. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) test and Bartlett’s sphericity test were used before EFA to investigate the sample size adequacy. The results of the KMO test (0.78) and Bartlett’s sphericity test (chi-square = 782.45; df = 66; P < 0.000) indicated data suitability for factor analysis.
Table 4 shows the findings of the factor analysis. Factor loads and variance explained the correlation of each item with the relevant subscales and descriptive data related to the extracted factors. Principal component analysis indicated that four factors could explain 55% of the variance. Table 4 shows factor load, eigenvalue, and percentage of variance explained. The EFA indicated that the four-factor solution accounted for 67.03% of the variance. Factor loadings ranged from 0.39 to 0.86. Four factors were created using varimax rotation (Table 5).
Factor Load, Eigenvalue, and Percentage of Explanation of Variance
Component | Total | Variance % | Cumulative % | Total | Variance % | Cumulative % | Total | Variance % | Cumulative % |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 3.179 | 26.491 | 26.491 | 3.179 | 26.491 | 26.491 | 2.285 | 19.042 | 19.042 |
2 | 1.448 | 12.066 | 38.556 | 1.448 | 12.066 | 38.556 | 1.625 | 13.545 | 32.587 |
3 | 1.056 | 8.803 | 47.359 | 1.056 | 8.803 | 47.359 | 1.545 | 12.876 | 45.463 |
4 | 1.008 | 8.397 | 55.755 | 1.008 | 8.397 | 55.755 | 1.235 | 10.293 | 55.755 |
Factor Load of the Items on Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale
Items | Factor 1 | Factor 2 | Factor 3 | Factor 4 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Items 1 | 0.81 | |||
Items 2 | 0.51 | |||
Items 3 | 0.64 | |||
Items 4 | 0.66 | |||
Items 5 | 0.67 | |||
Items 6 | 0.39 | |||
Items 7 | 0.65 | |||
Items 8 | 0.65 | |||
Items 9 | 0.54 | |||
Items 10 | 0.81 | |||
Items 11 | 0.86 | |||
Items 12 | 0.60 |
5. Discussion
The present study attempted to investigate the factor structure and psychometric properties of the SUIS-P to measure the tendency toward using mental imagery in everyday life. Convergent and discriminant validity was investigated to evaluate construct validity. Convergent validity analyses showed a moderate positive correlation between the SUIS and AST-D. On the other hand, to calculate discriminant validity, the BDI-II was selected.
The findings of previous research (22) were the present study’s rationale for choosing the BDI-II as a measure of discriminant validity. According to Gorgen’s study (23), there was no significant association between the tendency to imagine (the SUIS) and depressive symptoms (the BDI-II) (r = 0.11, P > 0.05). There was no significant correlation between the SUIS and depression as a discriminant or divergent validity. Compatible with the findings of Lang et al.’s study (22), the current study detected no significant correlation between the tendency to imagine and depression. However, depression symptoms and disorders are primarily specific, self-relevant, and verbal processes not directly recorded and addressed within the SUIS.
Several explanations can be considered for the lack of association between SUIS and BDI. Previous studies have shown that rumination processes play an essential role in intensifying and maintaining depressive symptoms (23, 24). This finding indicated that depression is more closely related to the verbal-ruminative than to an individual’s visual-imaginative style of thinking. In contrast to depressive symptomatology, the SUIS assesses the use of mental images about everyday situations or activities that are more neutral in terms of valence. In addition to this difference in valence, it would be possible that the symptoms of depression are less associated with the frequency or inclination toward mental images.
Empirical findings showed that individuals with symptoms of depression or a depressive disorder describe everyday or positive mental images less vividly than control participants (25-27). Instead, the SUIS asked about the general, affectively neutral, habitual use of mental images in everyday life. In line with the current study’s expectations and previous studies (13), the total score of the SUIS had a positive correlation with the AST-D (vividness subscale). Researchers have reported that individuals with excellent vividness capability scored higher on the SUIS than low-vividness individuals (4, 13). One reason behind the moderate correlation coefficient of the SUIS and AST-D is that the AST-D focuses mainly on the quality and vividness of mental imagery; nevertheless, the SUIS generally evaluates the generation of mental images.
The mean values obtained by the SUIS are comparable to the results obtained in other samples from the general population (15, 28). The mean values of the total score of the SUIS in the present study are consistent with those of previous studies (15, 29-31). The present study did not find any significant correlation between mental imagery use with education level, marriage status, and age. Although the difference between male and female participants in the total score of the SUIS was not statistically significant, the present study showed that female subjects achieved higher mean values than male subjects. Nevertheless, according to the literature review on this topic, there was no interpretation or explanation for this result (23). The finding is consistent with the findings of previous research (4). In addition, Tanaka et al. (32) in their study also demonstrated no significant difference in the total score of the SUIS between the genders.
This study showed that the SUIS-P was a reliable and valid self-report scale. The internal consistency of the SUIS was good (a = 0.75). Although the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the SUIS in the present study was lower than the original English version (a = 0.98) (13), it was higher than previous studies (a = 0.66) (23), (a = 0.63) (33), (a = 0.66) (32) (a = 0.72; a = 0.72; a = 0.76) (4). Contrary to the original English version, it was impossible to prove the one-dimensionality of the SUIS for the Persian version with the help of EFA. According to previous studies (4, 23, 32), the CFA of a one-factor model showed an acceptable comparative fit index (CFI), and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) to good (the root mean square error of approximation) model fit but lower CFI and TLI values, which could mean that a multi-factor model could also be fit for the SUIS.
In the present study, some items in the SUIS, including items 6, 1, and 4, had poor psychometric properties that were inadequate. Therefore, it is better to eliminate or modify them in future studies to promote the psychometric properties of the SUIS. If a short version of the SUIS is desired, items 1, 4, and 6 might be excluded from the Persian version.
In the scale instructions, it is up to the participants to decide which adopted perspective will affect the quality and vividness of mental images. Emotions are also influenced by the perspective adopted in mental imagery. Adopting a first-person (field) perspective is associated with more emotional states than a third-person (observer) perspective (2). According to the SUIS instructions, it is not clear to the participant how to imagine the scale’s situations. Therefore, the participant creates and manipulates mental images based on his/her tendency and discretion in the field perspective (first person) or observer perspective (third person).
This issue is important because previous studies (34) showed that negative interpretation bias is especially high when images are produced from a field perspective. On the other hand, in the intervention of positive mental imagery, when participants are asked to create imagery from a field perspective, they show more mood changes than when they are asked to create imagery from an observer perspective (35). Therefore, the instructions with which the participant completes the SUIS can be considered a confounding variable. Interestingly, most mental imagery disorders use an observer perspective (e.g., social anxiety disorder, agoraphobia, post-traumatic stress disorder, and depression) (2). In this regard, future studies are suggested to modify their instructions so that the participant imagines the situations based on the first-person (field) perspective.
Most previous studies revealed that the SUIS had a single-factor model (4, 32, 23), which suggested a fundamental component: the general use of visual mental images. However, a two-factor model was also identified by Nelis and Holmes (4). In addition, the fit indices in CFA were not wholly fit for the one-factor model. On the other hand, the fitness indices of CFA in the German and Japanese studies (23, 32) were not entirely fit with the one-factor model of the SUIS. In the present study, EFA showed four factors.
According to the previous findings (4, 23, 32), the SUIS is a one-dimensional scale; however, the findings of the current study extracted four factors. Therefore, this finding is not consistent with the findings of previous studies (4, 23, 32). Since these four factors were still observed in various factor analyses, several other reasons can be interpreted regarding this finding, including inappropriate sampling or careless answering of the participants, the sensitivity of the target structure, and the concern of the samples in responding to potential stigma.
The current study had several limitations. One of the limitations was the larger number of females than males, which might jeopardize the external validity or generalizability of the findings. Therefore, in future studies, this limitation should be overcome by quota sampling. Secondly, given that the EFA of the present study indicated four factors of the SUIS, to resolve this discrepancy in research findings, one of the research suggestions is to perform CFA with a larger sample. Thirdly, online data collection was another limitation. Although online data collection is very convenient and practical, it has been noted that sampling biases are most likely to occur in this data collection method. Fourthly, another limitation was that this study did not use a screening scale to detect psychiatric disorders.
5.1. Conclusions
According to the obtained findings, the SUIS-P has acceptable psychometric properties in the Iranian population to assess individual differences in the everyday use of mental imagery. Therefore, it can be utilized in clinical and research contexts. The SUIS is a promising instrument for further research into the association between mental imagery and cognitive (e.g., problem-solving) and emotional (e.g., emotion regulation) processes and psychological symptoms. In addition, it is a useful method for the applied tendency to imagine as a control variable (e.g., between different conditions) in mental imagery-based intervention, as Blackwell recommended in his study (36). Future studies can investigate and promote the psychometric properties of the SUIS, especially in clinical populations.
References
-
1.
Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Ganis G. The case for mental imagery. Oxford University Press; 2006.
-
2.
Ji JL, Kavanagh DJ, Holmes EA, MacLeod C, Di Simplicio M. Mental imagery in psychiatry: conceptual & clinical implications. CNS Spectr. 2019;24(1):114-26. [PubMed ID: 30688194]. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852918001487.
-
3.
Holmes EA, Mathews A. Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(3):349-62. [PubMed ID: 20116915]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.001.
-
4.
Nelis S, Holmes EA, Griffith JW, Raes F. Mental imagery during daily life: Psychometric evaluation of the Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS). Psychol Belg. 2014;54(1):19-32. [PubMed ID: 26290615]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4538780]. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.ag.
-
5.
Bibi A, Margraf J, Blackwell SE. Positive imagery cognitive bias modification for symptoms of depression among university students in Pakistan: A pilot study. J Exp Psychopathol. 2020;11(2):204380872091803. https://doi.org/10.1177/2043808720918030.
-
6.
Joormann J, Waugh CE, Gotlib IH. Cognitive Bias Modification for Interpretation in Major Depression: Effects on Memory and Stress Reactivity. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3(1):126-39. [PubMed ID: 25593790]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4290215]. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614560748.
-
7.
Renner F, Murphy FC, Ji JL, Manly T, Holmes EA. Mental imagery as a "motivational amplifier" to promote activities. Behav Res Ther. 2019;114:51-9. [PubMed ID: 30797989]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6416378]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.02.002.
-
8.
Marks DF. Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br J Psychol. 1973;64(1):17-24. [PubMed ID: 4742442]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x.
-
9.
Sheehan PW. A shortened form of Betts' questionnaire upon mental imagery. J Clin Psychol. 1967;23(3):386-9. [PubMed ID: 6082130].
-
10.
Martin KA, Moritz SE, Hall CR. Imagery Use in Sport: A Literature Review and Applied Model. Sport Psychol. 1999;13(3):245-68. https://doi.org/10.1123/tsp.13.3.245.
-
11.
Min H, Kwon SM, Lee SA. The Influence of Imagery Processing on the Experience of Pleasure and Anhedonic Depression. Korean J Clin Psychol. 2019;38(2):157-70. https://doi.org/10.15842/kjcp.2019.38.2.003.
-
12.
Kosslyn SM, Chabris CF, Shephard JM, Thompson WL. Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS). APA PsycTests. 1998. https://doi.org/10.1037/t57899-000.
-
13.
Reisberg D, Pearson DG, Kosslyn SM. Intuitions and introspections about imagery: the role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator's theoretical views. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2003;17(2):147-60. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.858.
-
14.
Blackwell SE, Rius-Ottenheim N, Schulte-van Maaren YW, Carlier IV, Middelkoop VD, Zitman FG, et al. Optimism and mental imagery: a possible cognitive marker to promote well-being? Psychiatry Res. 2013;206(1):56-61. [PubMed ID: 23084598]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3605581]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.09.047.
-
15.
McCarthy-Jones S, Knowles R, Rowse G. More than words? Hypomanic personality traits, visual imagery and verbal thought in young adults. Conscious Cogn. 2012;21(3):1375-81. [PubMed ID: 22850327]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.07.004.
-
16.
Pearson DG, Deeprose C, Wallace-Hadrill SM, Burnett Heyes S, Holmes EA. Assessing mental imagery in clinical psychology: a review of imagery measures and a guiding framework. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33(1):1-23. [PubMed ID: 23123567]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3545187]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.001.
-
17.
Holmes EA, Mathews A, Dalgleish T, Mackintosh B. Positive interpretation training: effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood. Behav Ther. 2006;37(3):237-47. [PubMed ID: 16942975]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.002.
-
18.
Holmes EA, Lang TJ, Deeprose C. Mental imagery and emotion in treatment across disorders: using the example of depression. Cogn Behav Ther. 2009;38 Suppl 1:21-8. [PubMed ID: 19697177]. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506070902980729.
-
19.
Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clini Psychol Rev. 1988;8(1):77-100. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5.
-
20.
Fata L, Birashk B, Atefvahid MK, Dabson KS. [Meaning assignment structures/schema, emotional states and cognitive processing of emotional information: comparing two conceptual frameworks]. Iran J Psychiatry Clin Psychol. 2005;11(3):312-26. Persian.
-
21.
Rohrbacher H, Reinecke A. Measuring change in depression-related interpretation bias: development and validation of a parallel ambiguous scenarios test. Cogn Behav Ther. 2014;43(3):239-50. [PubMed ID: 24903154]. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2014.919605.
-
22.
Lang TJ, Blackwell SE, Harmer CJ, Davison P, Holmes EA. Cognitive Bias Modification Using Mental Imagery for Depression: Developing a Novel Computerized Intervention to Change Negative Thinking Styles. Eur J Pers. 2012;26(2):145-57. [PubMed ID: 23316101]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3532611]. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.855.
-
23.
Görgen SM, Hiller W, Witthöft M. Die Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS)–Entwicklung und teststatistische Prüfung einer deutschen Adaption. Diagnostica. 2016;62(1):31-43. German. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000135.
-
24.
Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. Rethinking Rumination. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008;3(5):400-24. [PubMed ID: 26158958]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.
-
25.
Holmes EA, Lang TJ, Moulds ML, Steele AM. Prospective and positive mental imagery deficits in dysphoria. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(8):976-81. [PubMed ID: 18538304]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.04.009.
-
26.
Morina N, Deeprose C, Pusowski C, Schmid M, Holmes EA. Prospective mental imagery in patients with major depressive disorder or anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25(8):1032-7. [PubMed ID: 21783339]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3389342]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.06.012.
-
27.
Torkan H, Kalantari M, Neshat-Doost HT, Maroufi M, Talebi H. Visual imagery and rumination as cognitive representations in major depressive disorder: A preliminary study. Interdiscip J Contemp Res Bus. 2012;4:777-89.
-
28.
Holmes EA, Mathews A, Mackintosh B, Dalgleish T. The causal effect of mental imagery on emotion assessed using picture-word cues. Emotion. 2008;8(3):395-409. [PubMed ID: 18540755]. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.395.
-
29.
Berna C, Lang TJ, Goodwin GM, Holmes EA. Developing a measure of interpretation bias for depressed mood: An ambiguous scenarios test. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51(3):349-54. [PubMed ID: 21822348]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3149299]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.005.
-
30.
Davies C, Malik A, Pictet A, Blackwell SE, Holmes EA. Involuntary memories after a positive film are dampened by a visuospatial task: unhelpful in depression but helpful in mania? Clin Psychol Psychother. 2012;19(4):341-51. [PubMed ID: 22570062]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3532607]. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1800.
-
31.
Deeprose C, Holmes EA. An exploration of prospective imagery: the impact of future events scale. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2010;38(2):201-9. [PubMed ID: 20074386]. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465809990671.
-
32.
Tanaka Y, Yoshinaga N, Tsuchiyagaito A, Sutoh C, Matsuzawa D, Hirano Y, et al. Mental imagery in social anxiety disorder: the development and clinical utility of a Japanese version of the Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS-J). J Asia Pac Couns Psychotherapy. 2018;9(2):171-85. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507686.2018.1487455.
-
33.
Vassilopoulos SP, Moberly NJ. Cognitive Bias Modification in Pre-adolescent Children: Inducing an Interpretation Bias Affects Self-imagery. Cogn Ther Res. 2012;37(3):547-56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-012-9481-4.
-
34.
Burnett Heyes S, Pictet A, Mitchell H, Raeder SM, Lau JYF, Holmes EA, et al. Mental Imagery-Based Training to Modify Mood and Cognitive Bias in Adolescents: Effects of Valence and Perspective. Cognit Ther Res. 2017;41(1):73-88. [PubMed ID: 28239214]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5306169]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9795-8.
-
35.
Boland J, Riggs KJ, Anderson RJ. A brighter future: The effect of positive episodic simulation on future predictions in non-depressed, moderately dysphoric & highly dysphoric individuals. Behav Res Ther. 2018;100:7-16. [PubMed ID: 29107763]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.10.010.
-
36.
Blackwell SE, Browning M, Mathews A, Pictet A, Welch J, Davies J, et al. Positive Imagery-Based Cognitive Bias Modification as a Web-Based Treatment Tool for Depressed Adults: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3(1):91-111. [PubMed ID: 25984421]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4359210]. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614560746.