Abstract
Context:
There is evidence of the positive effect of neurofeedback, neuropsychology, and phonological awareness interventions on improving dyslexia symptoms. However, no agreement exists on the effectiveness of these interventions and the most efficient intervention.Objectives:
This research aimed to compare the effectiveness of interventions based on neurofeedback, neuropsychology, and phonological awareness in improving dyslexia symptoms in students with the network meta-analysis method to determine the most efficient intervention.Evidence Acquisition:
A network meta-analysis was conducted to identify studies on neurofeedback, neuropsychology, and phonological awareness interventions. A systematic review was done using Scopus, PubMed, Magiran, SID, and Civilica databases to find studies related to the research objectives, which led to the analysis of 49 studies with 1,741 participants and 15 types of interventions compared to controls. Individual interventions were classified into neurofeedback, neuropsychology, and phonological awareness. The studies were evaluated in terms of the risk of bias. The data were analyzed using the frequency approach through R and R studio software to compare the direct and indirect evidence of the effectiveness of the interventions.Results:
Neurofeedback, neuropsychology, and phonological awareness interventions were effective in improving dyslexia symptoms compared to controls. The perceptual-motor method, among individual interventions, and neuropsychological interventions, among group interventions, were ranked as "interventions with greater effectiveness" in reducing dyslexia symptoms. Subgroup analysis was also performed to find the sources of heterogeneity and inconsistency.Conclusions:
To the perceptual-motor method and neuropsychology intervention group were ranked as the best interventions.. However, most of these studies were conducted in Iran, and further studies in different cultural fields and countries can help clarify the issue.Keywords
1. Context
Reading disorder is the most common category among learning disorders. Various studies have shown that about 80% of children with learning disorders also have reading disorders (1). Dyslexic students face major problems in word recognition, spelling, handwriting, reading comprehension, understanding the meaning of words, and reading ability (2). Ortiz et al. (3) reported the prevalence of this disorder to be 5 - 12% using reading performance criteria cited in Peterson and Pennington (2012). Moreover, the prevalence of reading disorders among Iranian students was reported to be 4.58% (4). Considering the psychological and social consequences of the problems caused by differentiating children from normal children in children's lives (5), it is necessary to agree on a suitable solution based on research results.
Despite numerous treatments for dyslexia and various studies on the effectiveness of these treatments, there is still inconsistency between the results of primary studies. Farnia et al. (6) showed the effectiveness of neurofeedback in improving the memory of normal adults. However, according to Alkoby et al. (7) and Marzbani et al. (8), there is no conclusive evidence about the effectiveness of neurofeedback despite its general applications for the treatment of learning disorders, hyperactivity, dyslexia, anxiety, depression, epilepsy, and substance abuse. Cancer and Antonietti (9) studied interventions based on auditory processing about reading problems. They showed that auditory processing interventions, which are part of the group of neuropsychological interventions, have a positive effect on improving dyslexia symptoms, but the results are inconsistent. In a meta-analysis by Galuschka et al. (10) on the effectiveness of treatment approaches for dyslexic children and adolescents, the evidence indicated the lack of efficiency of three types of phonological awareness training in treating dyslexia. The meta-analysis by Lee and Yoon (11) regarding the effectiveness of repeated reading in the fluency of dyslexic students indicates the positive effect of this intervention. A systematic review by Peters et al. (12) on the effectiveness of visual and dynamic attention interventions in dyslexic children's reading showed that visual-perceptual training affects fluency and reading comprehension. However, most of these studies have been limited to pairwise and two-by-two evaluations in which one variable is compared with another. Network meta-analysis has the advantage of using available direct and indirect evidence. Empirical studies have shown that network meta-analysis provides more useful and accurate estimates of intervention effects than direct or indirect estimation alone (13). Network meta-analysis can provide useful information about comparisons between pairs of interventions that would never be evaluated in individual randomized interventions. Simultaneous comparison of all interventions considered in the same analysis allows estimation of their relative ranking on the outcome(s) (13).
2. Objectives
This network meta-analysis aimed to answer these two questions:
- Are neurofeedback, neuropsychology, and phonological awareness group interventions effective in improving dyslexia symptoms?
- Which individual intervention and which group intervention are more likely to reduce the symptoms of dyslexia?
3. Evidence Acquisition
In this research, a systematic review of interventions for dyslexia and a meta-analysis method was used in this review. We included studies in which the age of subjects was under 18 years, the full text was available, the study design was pretest-posttest with a control group, the effect sizes were reported, or the necessary data for calculating the effect sizes existed, and the research design was experimental or quasi-experimental. Accordingly, subjects with other disorders, such as hyperactivity, conduct, and reading disorders, were excluded from the research.
The terms "dyslexia," "reading disorder," "reading disability," "learning disorder," "learning disability," and "LD," or their Persian equivalents, were searched in Scopus, PubMed, Magiran, SID, and Civilica from 1991 to the end of 2021. Since some internal databases did not show sensitivity to the search operators (OR, AND, NOT), the search was conducted only through the keywords of reading disorder, reading disability, dyslexia, and learning disorder to achieve high sensitivity. The search for research sources was limited to English and Farsi. In addition, the reference lists of the selected articles were screened. Initial title screening was done by the first author using Mendeley software and a website. The first and second authors then independently screened the abstracts and entire articles from the initial screening and resolved disagreements using discussion, the results of which were confirmed by the third and fourth authors.
The interventions in this research consisted of neuropsychology, neurofeedback, and phonological awareness interventions, each with several interventions. Neuropsychological interventions comprised cognitive strategies training, metacognitive strategies training, hemisphere-specific stimulation, visual perception skills training, neuropsychology, accuracy training, working memory strategies training, Frostig visual perception, auditory comprehension training, attention training based on the Fletcher program, self-reinforcement, strengthening working memory with computers, perceptual-motor training, computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation, thinking maps training, cognitive games, linguistic games, two hemispheres training, auditory processing training, visual processing training, and non-verbal listening training. Phonological awareness interventions were phonological awareness, phonemic games, cognitive rehabilitation based on phonological awareness, morpheme awareness through games, phoneme-based active memory training, morphological awareness, and phonological training using a robot. Neurofeedback interventions consisted of neurofeedback training, biofeedback training, electroencephalography (EEG), hemoencephalography (HEG), or functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) of the Loretta type.
4. Data Extraction
At this stage, the full texts of the articles were reviewed. Data were extracted from studies using a form. The extracted data consisted of the name of the author(s), year of publication, data collection method, sample size, mean and standard deviation in the experimental and control groups, outcome(s), participants' gender, age, and education level, and quality of the study. Some of the most important characteristics of the selected studies are listed in Table 1. In the first stage of the search, 8,032 studies were obtained. Finally, based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria and removing irrelevant and repetitive studies, 49 articles entered the final stage.
Description of Studies Included in the Systematic Review
Study | Year | Country | Research Design | Sample Size | Mean Age | Intervention Type | Outcome | Tools | Quality Assessment |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baezzat et al. (14) | 2006 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with the control group | 28 | 10 | Neuropsychology | Reading accuracy; reading comprehension | Nasaft et al. reading test (14) | Good |
Same Siahkalroodi et al. (15) | 2009 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 60 | 9 | Visual perception skills training | Reading comprehension; improving reading performance | Fallahchay reading test (16) | Moderate |
Yaaghobi and Ahadi (17) | 2005 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 64 | Meta-cognitive strategies | Improving reading performance | Researcher-made reading test (17) | Good | |
Ghobari-Bonab et al. (18) | 2012 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | 11.5 | Meta-cognitive strategies | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Good |
Daemi (20) | 2012 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 68 | Meta-cognitive strategies | Improving reading performance; speed of reading | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Karimi and Askari (21) | 2013 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Working memory strategies training | Improving reading performance | Azizian and Abedi reading test (22) | Good | |
Karami et al. (23) | 2013 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 20 | Phonological processing | Speed of reading, reading comprehension, reading accuracy | Sima-Shirazi and Nili-Pour reading test (24) | Good | |
Vatandoost et al. (25) | 2013 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 20 | 8.5 | Auditory processing training | Improving reading performance | Azizian and Abedi reading test (22) | Good |
Jadidi Feighan et al. (26) | 2014 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 40 | Neuropsychology | Speed of reading, reading comprehension, reading accuracy | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Esfahani et al. (27) | 2014 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Meta-cognitive strategies | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Chupan Zideh et al. (28) | 2015 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Attention training | Improving reading performance | Bdyyan reading test (29) | Good | |
Yarmohammadian et al. (30) | 2015 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Cognitive strategies | Cognitive strategies | Bdyyan reading test (29) | Good | |
Mozami Goudarzi et al. (31) | 2015 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Amplification of working memory | Improving reading performance, reading accuracy | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Bayrami et al. (32) | 2015 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Neuropsychology | Reading comprehension, reading accuracy | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Narimani et al. (33) | 2015 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Phonological processing | Reading comprehension, reading accuracy, speed of reading | Shafiei et al. reading test (34) | Good | |
Nasri and Karimi Lichahi (35) | 2016 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 40 | Perceptual-motor training | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Hosseinkhanzadeh et al. (36) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | 10 | Cognitive rehabilitation | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Good |
Amrollahi Bioki and Hoosein Khanzadeh (37) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Self-learning technique | Reading comprehension, speed of reading | Yarmohammadian et al. reading test (30) | Good | |
Karami et al. (38) | 2016 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 32 | Meta-cognitive strategies | Reading comprehension, reading accuracy, speed of reading | Fallahchay reading test (16) | Good | |
Sheykholeslami et al. (39) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 40 | 9.2 | working memory training | Improving reading performance | NEMA test (19) | Good |
Rasouli et al. (40) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 40 | Meta-cognitive strategies | Reading comprehension, speed of reading | Sharifi and Raefi reading test (41) | Moderate | |
Hamidi and Fayazbakhsh (42) | 2016 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 60 | Working memory training | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Akbari et al. (43) | 2019 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 20 | 10 | Working memory training | Reading comprehension | Azizian and Abedi reading test (22) | Good |
Wang et al. (44) | 2019 | China | Pretest, posttest with control group | 46 | Visual and auditory temporal processing, training | Improving reading performance | Hung reading test (44) | Moderate | |
Vojoudi et al. (4) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 60 | Educational package based on visual-spatial processing | Improving reading performance | Moradi et al. reading test (19) | Good | |
Pape-Neumann et al. (45) | 2015 | Germany | Pretest, posttest with control group | 20 | 9.8 | Phonological processing | Improving reading performance, reading comprehension | Salzburg reading test (46) | Good |
Layes et al. (47) | 2020 | France | Pretest, posttest with control group | 44 | 10.25 | Phonological processing | Reading comprehension, reading accuracy | Layes et al. reading test (47) | Moderate |
Layes et al. (48) | 2019 | France | Pretest, posttest with control group | 40 | 10.32 | Visuomotor-based, intervention | Improving reading performance | Layes et al. reading test (47) | Moderate |
Wang (49) | 2017 | China | Pretest, posttest with control group | 56 | 8.2 | Phonological processing | Improving reading performance | Hung reading test (44) | Moderate |
Yaghoubi et al. (50) | 2013 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Visual perception skills training | Improving reading performance, reading comprehension | Fallahchay reading test (16) | Weak | |
Adib Sereshki et al. (51) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Programs based on attention | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Moderate | |
Sabaghi et al. (52) | 2016 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 36 | 9.5 | Neurofeedback | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Moderate |
Dehghan et al. (53) | 2018 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Cognitive plays | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Weak | |
Ladonni Fard et al. (54) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | 10 | Linguistic plays program | Reading comprehension, speed of reading | Sima-Shirazi and Nili-Pour reading test (24) | Moderate |
Rahbar Karbasdehi et al. (55) | 2018 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 27 | 10.11 | Self-regulation strategies | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Moderate |
Hashemi et al. (56) | 2019 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 60 | Cognitive rehabilitation focused on phonological awareness | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Weak | |
Safari et al. (57) | 2019 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | 9.66 | Cognitive rehabilitation | Reading comprehension, speed of reading | Nasaft et al. reading test (14) | Weak |
Naji et al. (58) | 2020 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Phonological active memory | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Moderate | |
Haghighatzadeh et al. (59) | 2020 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Perceptual-motor training | Improving reading performance | Bdyyan reading test (29) | Weak | |
Hemati Almdarloo and Tavakoli (60) | 2020 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Programs based on attention | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Moderate | |
Tikderi and Kafi (61) | 2020 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | 10.5 | Working memory training | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Weak |
Narimani et al. (62) | 2012 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 31 | 9.5 | Biofeedback | Reading comprehension | Shafiei et al. reading test (34) | Good |
Azami and Haj Sadeghi (63) | 2017 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Neurofeedback | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Moderate | |
Soleymani and Vakili (64) | 2016 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Cognitive rehabilitation | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Weak | |
Gharibi et al. (65) | 2022 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Programs based on attention | Reading comprehension | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Kaveh et al. (66) | 2021 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 20 | 8 | Auditory processing training | Reading comprehension | Researcher-made reading test (66) | Good |
Sabeghi et al. (67) | 2022 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | Neuropsychology | Reading comprehension improving reading performance | NEMA test (19) | Good | |
Safari et al. (68) | 2020 | Iran | Pretest, posttest with control group | 30 | 9.5 | Cognitive rehabilitation | Reading comprehension, speed of reading | Nasaft et al. reading test (14) | Moderate |
Pasqualotto and Venuti (69) | 2020 | Italy | Pretest, posttest with control group | 49 | 8.95 | Phonological processing | Speed of reading | Researcher-made reading test (69) | Moderate |
Risk of bias assessment: Bias in psychological interventions is essential due to using different measurement tools (70). This is certainly more evident in the network meta-analysis, where one assumption is the consistency of the studies. Therefore, to assess the risk of bias in the studies, the modified Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) tool was used (71), which contains five questions:
(1) Are the experimental and control groups adequately matched?
(2) Is the research implementation method clearly stated, for example, in the definition of the research population?
(3) Is the method of selecting research subjects random?
(4) Is the method of random selection stated?
(5) Is the assignment of subjects to experimental and control groups random?
Accordingly, a total score ranging from 0 to 10 is obtained for each study. Scores below 5, between 5 and 7, and 8 or above are considered poor, average, and good quality, respectively.
Statistical analysis: Network meta-analysis was performed using R and R studio software and the "netmeta" package via a random-effects model and frequency approach for the combination of direct and indirect evidence (72). Direct evidence refers to the pooled effect based on randomized studies (as in traditional meta-analysis), while indirect evidence is calculated from the network. For example, the difference between B and C is derived from the difference between A versus B and A versus C.
The network structure was created for both individual and group interventions. Network meta-analysis provides standardized mean difference effect sizes between groups based on both direct and indirect evidence, as well as confidence intervals and P values. Cohen's d interpretation was used to describe effect sizes: 0.2 (small), 0.5 (medium), and 0.8 (large) (73).
The consistency assumption was checked using the P-value statistic from the direct and indirect comparison of the standardized mean difference for each arm connected in the network and visual examination of the graph, and no evidence of inconsistency in the network was found. The analysis also provided information about the probability of each intervention being the most effective using surface under the cumulative rankings curve (SUCRA) values for each intervention and the three groups of interventions. The SUCRA value shows each intervention relative to the hypothetical best intervention (score out of 100) (72).
Evaluating the assumptions of network meta-analysis: One of the presuppositions of network meta-analysis is the examination of heterogeneity. Heterogeneity refers to the fact that there should not be dispersion between the results of studies in pairwise comparisons (74). Therefore, using the I2 statistic, the heterogeneity between the studies was checked, showing a value significant of 91.6. To find the reason for heterogeneity, the I2 value was reduced to 76.6 using the technique of removing outliers. Other factors influencing heterogeneity can root in using different outcome measurement tools with different scoring systems, the presence of a study with a high impact on research, and the different sample sizes of the studies. Therefore, the impact analysis method was used, but it did not reduce heterogeneity. A third step to find sources of heterogeneity was subgroup analysis based on study quality, which did not show evidence of reduced heterogeneity. Another (or the most important) assumption of network meta-analysis, without which it is practically impossible to implement network meta-analysis, is consistency or homogeneity. Therefore, Cochrane's q-test was used to evaluate this assumption, confirming the similarity of direct and indirect comparisons.
5. Results
5.1. Description of Included Studies
From the databases' search, 8,032 studies were obtained (Figure 1).
Prisma diagram for systematic review and meta-analysis
After removing duplicate and unrelated items and reviewing research titles and abstracts, 480 full-text studies remained. Finally, using the inclusion and exclusion criteria, 49 studies on the outcome of dyslexia (improving reading performance, reading comprehension, reading speed, and accuracy) with 1,741 participants (977 boys (56%) and 764 girls (44%)) were included in the systematic review. There were 48 interventions, control groups, and combined interventions that were classified into 14 classes. Subsequently, they were categorized into three groups for analysis: Neurofeedback-based, neuropsychological-based, and phonological awareness interventions. Of them, 43 studies were conducted in Iran, one in Italy, two in France, one in Germany, and two in China. All the included studies utilized a pretest-posttest design with a control group. Twenty-two studies used (NEMA) test Moradi et al. (19), three studies the reading test of Nesfat et al. (14), three studies the reading test of Fallahchay (16), four studies a researcher-made test, three studies the reading test of Azizian and Abedi (22), two studies the reading test of Sima-Shirazi and Nili-Pour (24), one study the reading test of Yarmohammadian et al. (30), three studies the reading test of Bdyyan (29), two studies the reading test of Shafiei et al. (34), one study the reading test of Sharifi and Raefi (41), two studies the reading test of Hung (44), one study the reading test of Moradi et al. (19), one study the Layes reading test (47), and one study the Salzburg reading test (46) as a study tool. Regarding risk of bias assessment, 26 studies were evaluated as good, 16 as average, and seven as poor (Table 1).
5.2. Analysis
A network map was created with 74 interventions from 49 studies (Figure 2). In the network structure, the lines indicate that two interventions are compared. The width of the lines indicates the number of studies that compared two interventions. The greater the number of studies comparing two interventions, the wider the line. There was no evidence of inconsistency in the models (Q = 1.20, df = 2, and P = 0.55). In other words, consistency means that the relative effect of a comparison (A-B) based on direct evidence is not different from the effect based on indirect evidence (13).
The structure of individual and group interventions. Vpst, visual perception skills training; Cmcs, cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies; Bs, brain stimulation; Wmst, working memory strategies training; Pp, phonological processing; Apt, auditory processing training; Epbvst, educational package based on visual-spatial processing; Nt, neuropsychological treatment; Pba, programs based on attention; Pmt, perceptual-motor training; Cr, cognitive rehabilitation; Neuro, neurofeedback; Cp, cognitive plays; Apt, auditory processing training; Lpp, linguistic plays program; Wait, control group.
The results of network meta-analysis comparisons (effect sizes and confidence intervals) showed that the effect sizes were statistically significant in the direct comparisons of the control group with visual perception skills training, motor perception training, attention-based programs, neuropsychological intervention, spatial perception processing-based training, cognitive games, and cognitive and metacognitive strategies. Moreover, the effect sizes were statistically significant in the indirect comparisons between working memory strategies and perceptual motor training, between neuropsychological intervention and spatial perception processing-based training, between visual perception skills training and phonological processing, linguistic games and spatial perception processing-based training, between phonological processing and motor perception training, attention programs, training based on spatial perception processing and cognitive and metacognitive strategies, between perceptual-motor training and neurofeedback, linguistic games, spatial perceptual processing-based training, cognitive and metacognitive strategies, and auditory processing. Furthermore, effect sizes were statistically significant between attention programs and linguistic games, spatial perception processing training and auditory processing, between neuropsychological intervention and neurofeedback, linguistic games, spatial perception processing-based training, cognitive rehabilitation, and cognitive and metacognitive strategies, between neurofeedback and spatial perception processing-based training, between linguistic games and cognitive and metacognitive strategies, and finally between training based on spatial perceptual processing and cognitive rehabilitation, cognitive games, cognitive and metacognitive strategies, and brain stimulation.
5.3. Ranking of Interventions
In the ranking of interventions, small effect sizes are considered “bad” or “weak.” Individual and group interventions were sorted based on P score, and the intervention or interventions with the largest effect size were placed at the top of the chart. According to the diagram in Figure 3, in individual interventions, the probability of obtaining the first rank in improving dyslexia symptoms for perceptual–motor training is 96%; after that, neuropsychological intervention and cognitive games were in the second and third places, respectively. In the analysis of three-group interventions, the probability that the neuropsychological group intervention will get the first rank in improving the symptoms of dyslexia is 97%. The neurofeedback and phonological awareness group interventions are ranked second and third in phonological awareness, respectively.
Ranking of individual and group interventions
6. Discussion and Conclusions
This study compares the effectiveness of neurofeedback, neuropsychological, and phonological awareness interventions in improving dyslexia symptoms using a network meta-analysis method. In this research, a total of 49 articles were examined. All the studies were assessed in terms of quality and risk of bias. Moreover, network meta-analysis assumptions, including consistency and homogeneity, were considered. Based on the findings, the consistency between the studies was acceptable, but the heterogeneity was high. Sources of heterogeneity were evaluated using three methods, and the reasons were mentioned in the section on heterogeneity.
Regarding the effectiveness of the three interventions, the results showed that the neurofeedback, neuropsychology, and phonological awareness group interventions effectively improve dyslexia symptoms. Based on the P score ranking, the first to third ranks in the individual interventions belonged to perceptual-motor training, neuropsychological interventions, and cognitive games, respectively. These results are somewhat consistent with the research of Peters et al. (12), Cancer and Antonietti (9), and Lee and Yoon (11). This effectiveness is due to the role of motor and perceptual games in creating changes in brain structures and creating changes in executive functions at a young age (75, 76). In-group interventions, neuropsychological group interventions, neurofeedback group interventions, and phonological awareness group interventions were ranked first to third, respectively. Accordingly, it can be stated that neuropsychological interventions are more likely to be effective in improving dyslexia symptoms than neurofeedback and phonological awareness interventions. Due to the high number of interventions in the field of dyslexia, these results can be a good guide for therapists in choosing the best and most effective treatment and intervention for reading disorders.
6.1. Research Limitations
One of the limitations of this research was the disparity in the number of studies in the three groups. Also, we only included studies in which the age of children was under 18 years, the full text was available, the design was pretest-posttest with a control group, the effect sizes were calculated, or the necessary data for calculating the effect sizes existed, and the research design was experimental or quasi-experimental. Accordingly, the subjects who simultaneously had other disorders, such as hyperactivity and conduct disorder, were excluded from the research. The number of initial studies was more in the neuropsychology group and less in the neurofeedback group because, due to the nature of the neurofeedback intervention and its expensive implementation, most studies in the neurofeedback group had single-subject designs and did not meet the conditions for inclusion in the study. Future studies should be carried out considering these limitations. Meta-analysis of single-sample results and conducting neurofeedback research in groups can also lead to a more accurate analysis.
References
-
1.
Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Frequency of reading, math, and writing disabilities in children with clinical disorders. Learn Individ Differ. 2006;16(2):145-57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2005.07.004.
-
2.
Fathi D, Faramarzi S, Eftekhar Saadi Z, Naderi F, Zargar Y. [Effectiveness of Targeted Reading Intervention Based on Response to Intervention on Reading Function and Academic Self-efficacy of Third Grade Elementary School Students with Dyslexia]. Pajouhan Sci J. 2022;20(1):24-32. Persian. https://doi.org/10.52547/psj.20.1.24.
-
3.
Ortiz A, Martinez-Murcia FJ, Luque JL, Gimenez A, Morales-Ortega R, Ortega J. Dyslexia Diagnosis by EEG Temporal and Spectral Descriptors: An Anomaly Detection Approach. Int J Neural Syst. 2020;30(7):2050029. [PubMed ID: 32496139]. https://doi.org/10.1142/S012906572050029X.
-
4.
Vojoudi K, Akhavan Tafti M, Khademi Ashkzari M. The Effectiveness of Educational Package Based on Visual-Spatial Processing in Reading Performance of Dyslexic Students. Avicenna J Neuro Psycho Physiol. 2017;4(3):79-86. https://doi.org/10.32598/ajnpp.4.3.79.
-
5.
Namdar P, Pourasghar M, Lal Alizadeh F, Shiva A. Anxiety, Depression, and Quality of Life in Caregivers of Children with Cleft Lip and Palate: A Systematic Review. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. 2022;16(2). e113591. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs-113591.
-
6.
Farnia S, Abedi-Darzi S, Fattahi S, Yazdani Charati J, Motamedi MR, Bakhshian F, et al. The Effect of Beta and Alpha Neurofeedback on Memory: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Sham-Controlled, Clinical Trial. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. 2017;11(2). e7431. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs.7431.
-
7.
Alkoby O, Abu-Rmileh A, Shriki O, Todder D. Can We Predict Who Will Respond to Neurofeedback? A Review of the Inefficacy Problem and Existing Predictors for Successful EEG Neurofeedback Learning. Neuroscience. 2018;378:155-64. [PubMed ID: 28069531]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.050.
-
8.
Marzbani H, Marateb HR, Mansourian M. Neurofeedback: A Comprehensive Review on System Design, Methodology and Clinical Applications. Basic Clin Neurosci. 2016;7(2):143-58. [PubMed ID: 27303609]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4892319]. https://doi.org/10.15412/J.BCN.03070208.
-
9.
Cancer A, Antonietti A. Music-based and auditory-based interventions for reading difficulties: A literature review. Heliyon. 2022;8(4). e09293. [PubMed ID: 35497042]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC9048091]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2022.e09293.
-
10.
Galuschka K, Ise E, Krick K, Schulte-Korne G. Effectiveness of treatment approaches for children and adolescents with reading disabilities: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS One. 2014;9(2). e89900. [PubMed ID: 24587110]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3935956]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089900.
-
11.
Lee J, Yoon SY. The Effects of Repeated Reading on Reading Fluency for Students With Reading Disabilities: A Meta-Analysis. J Learn Disabil. 2017;50(2):213-24. [PubMed ID: 26408529]. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219415605194.
-
12.
Peters JL, De Losa L, Bavin EL, Crewther SG. Efficacy of dynamic visuo-attentional interventions for reading in dyslexic and neurotypical children: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;100:58-76. [PubMed ID: 30802473]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.015.
-
13.
Higgins JP, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, et al. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2019. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119536604.
-
14.
Baezzat F, Banijamali S, Moazzemi D. [Effects of neuropsychological treatment on the reading efficiency of iranian students with developmental dyslexia of linguistic type]. Psychol Stud. 2006;2(1-2):107-24. Persian.
-
15.
Same Siahkalroodi L, Alizadeh H, Kooshesh M. [The Impact of Visual Perception Skills Training on Reading Performance in Students with Dyslexia]. Adv Cogn Sci. 2009;11(2):63-72. Persian.
-
16.
Fallahchay R. [Investigation of reading and writing disorders among elementary school students of Rasht city] [master's thesis]. Tehran: Tarbiat Modares University; 1995. Persian.
-
17.
Yaaghobi A, Ahadi H. [The effectiveness off metacognitive strategies instuction on 4th and 5th grade elementary dyslexia male students reading improvement]. Psychol Stud. 2005;1(1):47-58. Persian.
-
18.
Ghobari-Bonab B, Afrooz G, Hasanzadeh S, Bakhshi J, Pirzadi H. [The impact of teaching active metacognitive thinking-oriented strategies and self monitoring on realing comprehension of students with the reading difficulties]. J Learn Disabil. 2012;1(2):77-97. Persian.
-
19.
Moradi A, Hosaini M, Kormi Nouri R, Hassani J, Parhoon H. [Reliability and Validity of Reading and Dyslexia Test (NEMA)]. Adv Cogn Sci. 2016;18(1):22-34. Persian.
-
20.
Daemi HR. [Evaluating the Effectiveness of Metacognitive Strategies Training in Improving Reading, Comprehension and Reading Speed of Students]. Educ Psychol. 2013;8(24):2-28. Persian.
-
21.
Karimi S, Askari S. [The effectiveness of working memory strategies training on improvement of reading performance in students with dyslexia]. J Learn Disabil. 2013;3(1):79-90. Persian.
-
22.
Azizian M, Abedi MR. [Construction and Standardization of Reading Level Diagnostic Test for Third Grade Primary School Children]. Iran J Psychiatry Clin Psychol. 2006;11(4):379-87. Persian.
-
23.
Karami J, Abbasi Z, Zakei A. [The effect of phonological awareness training on speed, accuracy and comprehension of students with dyslexia]. J Learn Disabil. 2013;2(3):38-53. Persian.
-
24.
Sima-Shirazi T, Nili-Pour R. [Developing and Standardization of a Diagnostic Reading Test]. Arch Rehabil. 2004;5(1):7-11. Persian.
-
25.
Vatandoost N, Yarmohammadian A, Abedi A, Ghaziasgar N, Moghtadaie M. [The effect of auditory perception training on reading performance of the 8-9-year old female students with dyslexia: A preliminary study]. Aud Vestib Res. 2013;22(4):60-8. Persian.
-
26.
Jadidi Feighan M, Abedi A, Jamali Paghale S, Jadidi Feighan M. [Effectiveness of Neuropsychological Interventions on the Reading Components (Speed, Accuracy and Understanding) of Students with Dyslexic]. Res Clin Psychol Couns. 2014;4(1):115-34. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22067/ijap.v4i1.15793.
-
27.
Esfahani A, Asgarnejad AS, Ahadi H, Mousavi AM. [Effectiveness of the Meta-cognitive Trainings in the Reading Functions of the Third Grade Male Dyslexia Students of the Elementary Schools]. J Cognit Psychol. 2014;1(2):10-8. Persian.
-
28.
Chupan Zideh R, Abedi A, Pirooz Zijerdi M. [The effectiveness of training attention based on fletcher’s program on the reading performance of female student with dyslexia]. J Learn Disabil. 2015;4(1):36-48. Persian.
-
29.
Bdyyan SA. [Survey of prevalence of reading and writing disorders in bilingual elementary schools in the area of feradounshar] [thesis]. Khorasgan: Khorasgan Islamic Azad University; 1996. Persian.
-
30.
Yarmohammadian A, Ghamarani A, Seifi Z, Arfa M. [Effectiveness of cognitive strategies training on memory, reading performance and speed of information processing in students with dyslexia]. J Learn Disabil. 2015;4(4):101-17. Persian.
-
31.
Mozami Goudarzi S, Arabani Dana A, Farokhi N, Goudarzi K. [Investigating the Amplification of Working Memory Using Computer and its Effectiveness on Recovering the Reading Function and Accuracy in Tehran's Dyslexic Students at Primary Level]. J Except Child. 2015;15(3):19-30. Persian.
-
32.
Bayrami M, Movahedi Y, Esmaili S, Zorahim R. [The effectiveness of neuropsychological rehabilitation on reading speed, accuracy of reading and reading comprehension in students with Dyslexia]. Iran J Rehabil Res Nurs. 2016;2(3):69-77. Persian.
-
33.
Narimani M, Nori R, Abolghasemi A. [Comparison of the effectiveness of phonological awareness strategies and Fernald multi-sensory to improve reading skill and comprehension of dyslexia students]. J Learn Disabil. 2015;4(3):104-20. Persian.
-
34.
Shafiei B, Tavakol S, Alinia L, Maracy MR, Sedaghati L, Foroughi R. [Developing a screening inventory reading test (IRT) for the Isfahanian students of the first to fifth grade]. Auditory and Vestibular Research. 2009;17(2):53-60. Persian.
-
35.
Nasri S, Karimi Lichahi R. [The comparison of the effectiveness of multi-sensory and perceptual-motor training method in improving reading skills dyslexic students]. J Learn Disabil. 2016;5(4):123-40. Persian.
-
36.
Hosseinkhanzadeh A, Latif Zanjani M, Taher M. [Efficacy of Computer-Assisted Cognitive Remediation (CACR) on Improvement Executive Functions and Reading Performance of Students with Dyslexia]. Neuropsychology. 2017;2(7):27-46. Persian.
-
37.
Amrollahi Bioki F, Hoosein Khanzadeh AA. [Self-instructional techniques effectiveness on reading performance and academic self-efficacy in dyslexic students]. Psychol Except Individ. 2017;7(25):53-73. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22054/jpe.2017.20839.1532.
-
38.
Karami J, Momeni K, Abbasi Z. [The effectiveness of metacognitive strategies and working memory training on reading performance (accuracy, speed and comprehension) of students with dyslexia]. Psychol Achiev. 2016;23(2):51-68. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22055/psy.2017.12621.
-
39.
Sheykholeslami A, Bakhshayesh AR, Barzegar-Bafrooei K, Moradi Ajami V. [The Effectiveness of Working Memory Training on Reading Performance and Memory Capacity of Students with Reading Disability]. J Clin Psychol. 2017;9(2):47-58. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22075/jcp.2017.9475.
-
40.
Rasouli M, Chobdari A, Kargar H, Rostami S. [Cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies to improve reading performance and academic self-efficacy in boys with dyslexia]. Middle East J Disabil Stud. 2018;8(0):32. Persian.
-
41.
Sharifi A, Raefi MH. [Introduction to pervasive disorders of development]. Tehran: Ghomes; 2009. Persian.
-
42.
Hamidi F, Fayazbakhsh M. [Effectiveness of Working Memory Training on Improving Reading Skills in Dyslexic Primary School Students]. J Instr Eval. 2015;9(35):13-35. Persian.
-
43.
Akbari E, Soltani-Kouhbanani S, Khosrorad R. The effectiveness of working memory computer assisted program on executive functions and reading progress of students with reading disability disorder. Electron J Gen Med. 2019;16(2):em123. https://doi.org/10.29333/ejgm/94044.
-
44.
Wang LC, Liu D, Xu Z. Distinct effects of visual and auditory temporal processing training on reading and reading-related abilities in Chinese children with dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. 2019;69(2):166-85. [PubMed ID: 30671864]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-019-00176-8.
-
45.
Pape-Neumann J, Ermingen-Marbach M, Grande M, Willmes K, Heim S. The role of phonological awareness in treatments of dyslexic primary school children. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars). 2015;75(1):80-106. [PubMed ID: 25856525].
-
46.
Mayringer H, Wimmer H. [Salzburg Screening for Reading in Grade 1-4, SLS 1-4]. Bern: Hans Huber; 2003. German.
-
47.
Layes S, Guendouz M, Lalonde R, Rebai M. Combined Phonological Awareness and Print Knowledge Training Improves Reading Accuracy and Comprehension in Children with Reading Disabilities. Intl J Disabil Dev Educ. 2022;69(4):1185-99. https://doi.org/10.1080/1034912x.2020.1779914.
-
48.
Layes S, Chouchani MS, Mecheri S, Lalonde R, Rebaï M. Efficacy of a visuomotor‐based intervention for children with reading and spelling disabilities: a pilot study. Br J Spec Educ. 2019;46(3):317-39. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8578.12278.
-
49.
Wang LC. Effects of Phonological Training on the Reading and Reading-Related Abilities of Hong Kong Children with Dyslexia. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1904. [PubMed ID: 29163285]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5671595]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01904.
-
50.
Yaghoubi A, Mohagheghi H, Ghafoori Asar M, Rashid K. [Comparison of the Effects of Fernald Multi-Sensory and Frostig Visual Perception Methods on Improvement of Reading Performance in Dyslexic Male Students in Hamedan City]. Psychol Except Individ. 2013;3(9):21-32. Persian.
-
51.
Adib Sereshki N, Moradi N, Yadegari F, Kannani Z. [The effectiveness of attention training on the reading performance of students with dyslexia]. J Cogn Psychol. 2017;4(4):61-70. Persian.
-
52.
Sabaghi A, Rezaee M, Mirzakhani N, Irani A, Tabatabaee SM. [Effectiveness of Neurofeedback Training on Reading Performanceand Working Memory in Students with Dyslexia]. Sci J Rehabil Med. 2017;6(3):11-20. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22037/jrm.2017.1100333.
-
53.
Dehghan N, Faramarzi S, Nadi MA, Arefi M. [Effect of Educational Interventions through Cognitive Plays on the Reading Performance of Dyslexic Students]. Middle East J Disabil Stud. 2017;7(0):87. Persian.
-
54.
Ladonni Fard N, Shojaee S, Hemmati Alamdarloo G. [The Effectiveness of linguistic plays program on Reading Performance of Male Students with dyslexia]. Psychol Except Individ. 2017;7(25):35-52. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22054/jpe.2017.6033.1182.
-
55.
Rahbar Karbasdehi F, Hosseinkhanzadeh A, Abolghasemi A. [Effects of Self-Regulation Strategies Training on Academic Self-Efficacy and Reading Performance of Students with Dyslexia]. Q J Fam Res. 2019;15(4):49-66. Persian.
-
56.
Hashemi T, Bayrami M, Esmaeilpour K, Nemati Sogolitappeh F, Khosheghbal M. [Comparison of the effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation focused on phonological awareness and cognitive rehabilitation focused on working memory in improving symptoms of dyslexia in primary school students]. J Learn Disabil. 2019;9(1):138-57. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22098/jld.2019.837.
-
57.
Safari N, Baezzat F, Ghaffari M. [The effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation on reading efficiency in dyslexic students]. J Psychol Stud. 2019;15(3):105-22. Persian.
-
58.
Naji ES, Shokoohi-Yekta M, Hasanzadeh S, Hejazi E, Ejei J. [The Effectiveness of Phoneme-Based Working Memory Education on Improving the Ability of Reading in Dyslexic Students]. J Psychol. 2020;3(24):299-316. Persian.
-
59.
Haghighatzadeh R, Ghamarani A, Faramarzi S. [Effect of -motor-perception training on reading performances of dyslexian elementary students of Isfahan]. J Health Syst Res. 2020;15(4):272-9. Persian. https://doi.org/10.48305/hsr.2020.15.4.104.
-
60.
Hemati Almdarloo G, Tavakoli F. [The Efficacy of Attention Plays on Reading Performance of Female Students with Dyslexia]. J Educ Psychol Stud. 2020;17(40):53-34. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22111/jeps.2021.5897.
-
61.
Tikderi A, Kafi H. [Effect of Working Memory Training on the Improving Reading Performance and Working Memory Capacity in Children with Dyslexia]. Iran J Pediatric Nurs. 2020;7(1):50-61. Persian.
-
62.
Narimani M, Abolghasemi A, Rajab S, Nazari MA, Zahed A. [The Impact of EEG Neurobiofeedback on Dyslexia Symptoms]. J Except Child. 2012;12(1):25-32. Persian.
-
63.
Azami E, Haj Sadeghi Z. [Comparing the effectiveness of the neurofeedback and Davis treatment methods on the reading performance of dyslexic students]. Zanko J Med Sci. 2017;18(58):1-14. Persian.
-
64.
Soleymani M, Vakili S. [Effectiveness of Cognitive Rehabilitation on the Promotion of Reading and Working Memory in Students with Reading Learning Difficulties]. Appl Psychol Res Q. 2016;7(1):151-62. Persian.
-
65.
Gharibi H, Ahmadi B, Mahmoodi H, Karimian E, Gharibi J. [The Effectiveness of Attention Games on Reading Ability of Dyslexic Children]. J Learn Disabil. 2022;11(3):90-102. Persian.
-
66.
Kaveh A, Hassan Zadeh R, Mirzaeian B. [The Effectiveness of Auditory Transformation Therapy with a Cognitive InformationProcessing Approach in the Treatment of Cognitive Dyslexia]. J Paramed Sci Rehabil. 2021;10(2):58-71. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22038/jpsr.2021.50461.2141.
-
67.
Sabeghi F, Mohammadyfar M, Rezaei A. Effectiveness of Neuropsychological Intervention on Reading Performance and Executive Functions in Dyslexic Children. Iran Evolut Educ Psychol. 2022;4(1):13-24. https://doi.org/10.52547/ieepj.4.1.13.
-
68.
Safari N, Baezzat F, Ghaffari M. [Effectiveness of Cognitive Rehabilitation Program on Attention Dimensions and Reading Efficacy in Students with Dyslexia]. J Child Ment Health. 2020;7(3):167-81. Persian. https://doi.org/10.52547/jcmh.7.3.14.
-
69.
Pasqualotto A, Venuti P. A Multifactorial Model of Dyslexia: Evidence from Executive Functions and Phonological‐based Treatments. Learn Disabil Res Pract. 2020;35(3):150-64. https://doi.org/10.1111/ldrp.12228.
-
70.
Munder T, Barth J. Cochrane's risk of bias tool in the context of psychotherapy outcome research. Psychother Res. 2018;28(3):347-55. [PubMed ID: 29224503]. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2017.1411628.
-
71.
Moola S, Munn Z, Sears K, Sfetcu R, Currie M, Lisy K, et al. Conducting systematic reviews of association (etiology): The Joanna Briggs Institute's approach. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2015;13(3):163-9. [PubMed ID: 26262566]. https://doi.org/10.1097/XEB.0000000000000064.
-
72.
Rücker G, Krahn U, König J, Efthimiou O, Davies A, Papakonstantinou T. Network Meta-Analysis using Frequentist Methods. 2022. Available from: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/netmeta/netmeta.pdf.
-
73.
Schünemann HJ, Vist GE, Higgins JP, Santesso N, Deeks JJ, Glasziou P, et al. Interpreting results and drawing conclusions. In: Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, et al., editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2019. p. 403-31. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119536604.ch15.
-
74.
Deeks JJ, Higgins JP, Altman DG. Analysing data and undertaking meta‐analyses. In: Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, et al., editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2019. p. 241-84. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119536604.ch10.
-
75.
Ahangar Ghorbani Z, Hajloo N, Sepehri Nasab Z, Moazez R. [The effectiveness of working memory on academic performance of students with specific learning disorder: A meta-analysis study]. J Learn Disabil. 2019;8(4):7-26. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22098/jld.2019.819.
-
76.
Basharpoor S, Seif E, Narimani M. [Systematic review of studies related to executive functions in children with dyslexia in the Iranian studies (2001-2018)]. J Learn Disabil. 2022;11(2):33-46. Persian. https://doi.org/10.22098/jld.2022.6083.1645.