Logo

Comparative Analysis of Executive Function and Sensory Processing in Adults with and Without Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; Their Impact on Participation: A Cross-Sectional Study

Author(s):
Hanieh Kazemy-PourHanieh Kazemy-PourHanieh Kazemy-Pour ORCID1, Mahnaz Hejazi-ShirmardMahnaz Hejazi-ShirmardMahnaz Hejazi-Shirmard ORCID1, Marzieh PashmdarfardMarzieh PashmdarfardMarzieh Pashmdarfard ORCID1, Sadaf Khojaste-FarSadaf Khojaste-Far1, Navid Mirzakhani AraghiNavid Mirzakhani AraghiNavid Mirzakhani Araghi ORCID1,*
1Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Rehabilitation, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran


IJ Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences:Vol. 19, issue 1; e158769
Published online:Mar 31, 2025
Article type:Research Article
Received:Dec 17, 2024
Accepted:Mar 19, 2025
How to Cite:Hanieh Kazemy-PourMahnaz Hejazi-ShirmardMarzieh PashmdarfardSadaf Khojaste-FarNavid Mirzakhani AraghiComparative Analysis of Executive Function and Sensory Processing in Adults with and Without Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; Their Impact on Participation: A Cross-Sectional Study.Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci.19(1):e158769.https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs-158769.

Abstract

Background:

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that can lead to difficulties in various aspects of adult participation.

Objectives:

This present study aimed to investigate executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities among adults with and without ADHD. Additionally, the study explored the relationships between executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities in adults with ADHD.

Methods:

In this cross-sectional study, 70 adults aged 18 to 40 with ADHD and 68 healthy peers were recruited from various clinics and centers across Tehran. Executive function and sensory processing were assessed using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult version (BRIEF-A) and the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile Questionnaire (AASP), respectively. Participation in meaningful activities was evaluated using the engagement in meaningful activity survey (EMAS). Independent samples t-tests were used to compare adults with ADHD to healthy adults. Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients were utilized to explore correlations. A stepwise linear regression model was applied to assess the predictive effects of various variables.

Results:

Significant differences were observed in executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities between adults with and without ADHD. Significant correlations were found between participation scores and all subscales of executive function and sensory processing.

Conclusions:

The findings indicate that individuals with ADHD exhibit significant differences in executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities compared to their non-ADHD peers. Executive function and sensory processing in adults with ADHD appear to impact their participation in meaningful activities. Further studies are needed to confirm these findings.

1. Background

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) represents a neurodevelopmental disorder that generally manifests during childhood (1) but frequently persists into adulthood (2). Three principal manifestations of this disorder — attention deficit, hyperactivity, and impulsivity — can profoundly impact various aspects of an adult’s life (3). Individuals diagnosed with ADHD often experience considerable impairments in academic performance, occupational status, daily functioning, and aspects of social functioning (4). In other words, their participation is negatively influenced in various aspects. The neuropsychological performance of adults with ADHD is of great importance, as notable correlations have been identified between essential and higher-order cognitive functions within this group (5). Selective attention, vigilance, inhibition, and interference control are significantly compromised in a substantial proportion of individuals affected by ADHD (5). Executive function consists of five primary domains: Fluency, planning, working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting (6). Different aspects of executive function can be influenced by the persistence of ADHD symptoms, ultimately resulting in behavioral challenges and negative functional consequences (7).

Sensory processing is a multisensory function through which environmental inputs are collected, processed, and integrated by the central nervous system, thereby facilitating appropriate and effective responses to one’s surroundings (8). Several studies have suggested the potential effects of sensory processing on various aspects of daily functioning and overall quality of life (9). Sensory processing also has considerable impacts on higher-order cognitive functions and adaptive behaviors (10). It is evident that sensory processing is a significant concern in ADHD, as a systematic review has indicated sensory gating and impaired sensory inhibition in adults with ADHD (10). Furthermore, adults with ADHD also reported significantly higher levels of hyper- and hypo-sensitivity in comparison to their non-ADHD counterparts (11).

Recent studies highlight the associations between neuropsychological deficits — including executive functions, delay-related behaviors, and emotion dysregulation — and their influence on everyday functioning and overall quality of life (12, 13).

2. Objectives

Additionally, evidence points to sensory processing challenges in adults with ADHD, underscoring the need to explore possible associations between neuropsychological deficits, sensory processing, and engagement in daily activities within this population. Therefore, the present study aimed to investigate the relationships between sensory processing, executive function, and participation in meaningful activities, with the goal of deepening our understanding of the associations between executive function, sensory processing, and involvement in meaningful activities among adults with ADHD.

3. Methods

Seventy adults with ADHD and 68 non-ADHD adults, aged 18 to 40 years, were recruited for this cross-sectional study from various clinics and centers across Tehran. Participants were selected using the convenience sampling method between January to December 2023. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Adults with ADHD were included in the study if they met all of the following inclusion criteria: Diagnosis of ADHD by a psychiatrist; age between 18 and 40 years; no intake of psychotropic medications on the assessment day; no significant cognitive impairments [mini-mental state examination (MMSE) score of ≥ 23]; ability to understand instructions and complete evaluation forms; and no neurological conditions, including stroke, seizures, or epilepsy. Non-ADHD adults were required to meet all the inclusion criteria except for the first criterion, which was the diagnosis of ADHD. The present study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences (IR.SBMU.RETECH.REC.1401.505), and all participants signed the informed consent form before evaluations.

3.1. Instruments

Demographic characteristics of participants were collected using a demographic questionnaire. Executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities were then evaluated.

3.1.1. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult version (BRIEF-A) is an assessment tool used to evaluate executive function in adults over the age of 18. It considers the everyday behavioral manifestations of executive dysfunction. The self-report form of BRIEF-A comprises 75 items across 9 different clinical scales, including two indexes: The Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) and the Metacognition Index (MI). The BRI includes inhibition, shift, emotional control, and self-monitoring. The MI is composed of initiation, working memory, planning/organizing, task monitoring, and material organization, which are scored on a 3-point scale (never, sometimes, often). The sum of the total scores represents the global executive composite (GEC). A higher total GEC indicates more severely impaired executive function (14). The BRIEF-A has good test-retest reliability (15).

3.1.2. Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile

The sensory profile provides a sensory history of an individual’s responses to everyday sensory experiences. It assesses sensory thresholds on a continuum, anchored by the four outermost points of each scale: "Low Registration", "Sensation Seeking", "Sensory Sensitivity" and "Sensation Avoiding". The Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile Questionnaire (AASP) can be completed in 15 to 20 minutes and consists of 60 items with a 5-point Likert scale response format (from 1 = rarely to 5 = almost always). Higher or lower scores indicate a higher or lower frequency of specific sensory responses in six sensory domains: Visual, auditory, taste, smell, touch, body position, and movement. It also encompasses two behavioral classifications: Emotional/social and activity level (16, 17). The Cronbach’s alpha score for different parts of the AASP ranges from 0.894 to 0.916 (P < 0.001) (17).

3.1.3. Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey

The engagement in meaningful activity survey (EMAS) assesses the significance of activities for individuals, focusing on several key aspects. It emphasizes aligning an activity with one’s personal value system and needs. Additionally, it evaluates how well an activity demonstrates competence and mastery and its importance within one’s social and cultural group. The items in the EMAS encapsulate various propositions from occupational therapy and occupational science that focus on the components of meaningful engagement. The EMAS consists of 12 items, scored using a Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Higher scores indicate a better quality of life and higher psychological well-being (18, 19). The Cronbach’s alpha score of the EMAS is 0.95 (19).

3.2. Statistical Analysis

In this study, continuous variables were expressed as mean ± standard deviation, while categorical variables were reported as frequency (percentage). A univariable analysis was conducted to explore differences in executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities between adults diagnosed with ADHD and those without the condition. Independent samples t-tests were employed to assess these differences. Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients were utilized to examine the relationships between BRIEF-A and AASP scores and EMAS scores. Given that some test subscales demonstrated a normal distribution while others showed a non-normal distribution, two distinct types of correlation coefficients were used based on the normality or non-normality of the paired variables. In the multivariable analysis, a stepwise linear regression model was employed to explore the predictive relationship between BRIEF-A and AASP scores with EMAS scores. Data analysis was conducted using SPSS for Windows, version 26.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA), and statistical significance was set at P < 0.05.

4. Results

The demographic characteristics of adults with and without ADHD are presented in Table 1. The mean age of participants in the ADHD group (n = 70) was 28.05 ± 5.19 years; 51.4% were female, and 48.6% were male. The non-ADHD group included 68 adults with a mean age of 29.41 ± 6.39 years; 50% were male, and 50% were female.

Table 1.Demographic Characteristics a
VariablesADHD (n = 70)Non-ADHD (n = 68)P-Value
Age28.05 ± 5.1929.41 ± 6.390.174
Gender0.868
Female36 (51.4)34 (50)
Male34 (48.6)34 (50)
Education status0.987
High school15 (21.4)8 (11.8)
Undergraduate36 (51.4)48 (70.6)
Graduate19 (27.1)12 (17.6)
Occupation0.504
Employed48 (68.6)40 (58.8)
Housewife3 (4.3)7 (10.3)
Student17 (24.3)21 (30.9)
Unemployed2 (2.9)0

Demographic Characteristics a

To determine the differences between adults with and without ADHD in executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities, an independent samples t-test was performed. These differences were statistically significant for BRIEF-A (GEC P ≤ 0.05, BRI P ≤ 0.05, MI P ≤ 0.05), AASP (Low registration P ≤ 0.05, sensation seeking P ≤ 0.05, sensory sensitivity P ≤ 0.05, sensation avoiding P ≤ 0.05), and EMAS (P ≤ 0.05). The mean, standard deviation, and P-value scores of the independent samples t-test are presented in Table 2. The results indicate significant differences in all executive function variables, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities between the ADHD and non-ADHD groups.

Table 2.Mean, Standard Deviation and Independent Sample t-test Results Between the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Group and Non-attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Group in Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version, Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile, and Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey a
Variables and SubscalesADHD (n = 70)Non-ADHD (n = 70)P-Value b
BRIEF-A
GEC159.67 ± 22.65111.76 ± 21.090.000
BRI65.4 ± 9.0445.94 ± 9.260.000
MI86.65 ± 13.9962.44 ± 13.490.000
AASP
Low registration37.95 ± 8.4531.63 ± 6.940.000
Sensation seeking48.77 ± 6.7244.54 ± 9.310.003
Sensory sensitivity43.72 ± 9.4936.17 ± 8.210.000
Sensation avoiding43.81 ± 8.2837.27 ± 7.750.000
EMAS41.24 ± 7.9746.63 ± 6.380.000

Mean, Standard Deviation and Independent Sample t-test Results Between the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Group and Non-attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Group in Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version, Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile, and Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey a

The findings from the four sensory processing subscales indicated that the mean scores of adults with ADHD exhibit atypical patterns in low registration, sensory sensitivity, and sensation avoiding. These individuals fell into a range that was "higher than what is observed in most people". In contrast, non-ADHD individuals were "similar to most people" in all four sensory processing indices.

The correlation coefficients between BRIEF-A and AASP with EMAS scores in the ADHD group are presented in Table 3. The results showed a significant correlation between the EMAS and BRIEF-A subscales, including the GEC (r = -0.390, P ≤ 0.05), BRI (r = -0.467, P ≤ 0.05), and MI (r = -0.425, P ≤ 0.05) scores. The results also revealed that all four sensory processing subscales significantly correlate with the EMAS. A weak correlation was found between the EMAS and three sensory processing subscales: Low registration (r = -0.358, P = 0.002), sensation seeking (r = -0.368, P = 0.002), and sensory sensitivity (r = -0.322, P = 0.007), and a moderate correlation between the EMAS and sensation avoiding (r = -0.415, P < 0.001) was discovered.

Table 3.The Correlation Coefficients Between Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version and Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile Variables with Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey Scores in Adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Executive Function and Sensory Processing EMAS
rP a
GEC-0.3900.001
BRI-0.4670.000
MI-0.4250.000
Low registration-0.3580.002
Sensory seeking-0.3670.002
Sensory sensitivity-0.3220.007
Sensory avoiding-0.4150.000

The Correlation Coefficients Between Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version and Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile Variables with Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey Scores in Adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

The results of the linear regression analysis are shown in Table 4. A stepwise linear regression model was employed to explore the predictive effects of the BRIEF-A and AASP scores on participation in meaningful activities among adults with ADHD. Behavioral Regulation Index and low registration were included as two predictive factors of participation (R2 = 0.277, BRI β = -0.400, P ≤ 0.05; low registration β = -0.237, P ≤ 0.05) in the linear regression analysis.

Table 4.The Result of the Hierarchical Multiple Linear Regressions in Adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
RR SquareAdjusted R SquareP-Value a
0.5260.2770.2550.000

The Result of the Hierarchical Multiple Linear Regressions in Adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

5. Discussion

In the present study, we aimed to compare executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities in adults with ADHD with their non-ADHD peers and investigate the relationship between executive function and sensory processing with involvement in meaningful activities. Our findings indicated significant differences in executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities among adults with ADHD compared to non-ADHD adults. Additionally, we found significant correlations between executive function and sensory processing with involvement in meaningful activities in adults with ADHD. Moreover, both executive function and sensory processing demonstrated a predictive effect on the participation of adults with ADHD.

Executive function is a fundamental neuropsychological function that can be impaired in neurodevelopmental disorders (20). Many studies have investigated the relationship between ADHD and executive function deficits, yielding significant results. Additionally, it has been proven that executive function disorders can predict symptoms of ADHD (21). Our findings showed that individuals with ADHD have poorer performance in executive function compared to non-ADHD adults.

An important question we sought to answer was the potential relationship between executive deficits and participation in meaningful activities of adults with ADHD. Findings from similar studies involving adolescents with ADHD indicate that deficits in executive function significantly impact their performance in complex cooking tasks (22). Research suggests that executive function in later adulthood plays a crucial role in helping individuals navigate obstacles, fostering a sense of control over their lives (23). This sense of control is closely linked to greater life satisfaction and positive affect, mainly through active participation in daily life functions (23). Additionally, correlations between executive function, quality of life, and depressive or anxiety symptoms have been suggested in recent studies (24). Our findings showed a moderate correlation between all three subscales of BRIEF-A and the EMAS. This finding, which shows a significant relationship between executive function and participation in meaningful activities, is consistent with studies conducted in this area. Considering the nature of EMAS, which assesses the subjective aspects of involvement, it can be concluded that executive function affects individuals’ sense of participation, life satisfaction, and understanding of environmental control.

Sensory processing in adults with ADHD has been examined in some studies. Results from studies investigating the sensory profiles of individuals with and without ADHD have highlighted significant differences in sensory processing patterns. Specifically, adults with ADHD demonstrated increased levels of both hyper-sensitivity and hypo-sensitivity compared to their peers without the condition (11, 25). Our findings were also aligned with results obtained in similar studies. We found that individuals with ADHD had significant differences in sensory processing patterns compared to their healthy peers and often fell into a category described as "more than most people". In contrast, non-ADHD peers are usually placed into a category defined as "similar to most people" in all sensory subscales, indicating that their sensory processing patterns are similar to most normal adults.

We examined the potential impacts of sensory processing problems on an individual’s participation. The relationship between sensory processing and participation in adults with ADHD is an unexplored area in research. Sensory processing can affect an individual’s daily functioning and social interactions. Similar studies on other populations, including individuals with serious mental illness, those with neurological problems, and older adults, have shown that sensory processing issues affect these individuals’ participation in occupations (26-28). Additionally, high sensitivity may negatively impact quality of life and daily functioning (9). The present study showed a weak to moderate correlation between all four sensory processing patterns — low registration, sensation seeking, sensory sensitivity, and sensation avoidance — and the EMAS.

Studies have demonstrated that individuals with ADHD encounter significant challenges across various domains of life, including family relationships, life skills, social skills, and academic performance (7), and these problems increase with age (29). The results of our study indicated that participation in meaningful activities was significantly different between adults with ADHD and their non-ADHD peers. The EMAS is one of the measures that assesses the degree of congruency of individuals’ participation with their values. Therefore, it can be concluded that the symptoms of ADHD can affect the subjective aspects of individuals’ involvement and, in turn, influence their satisfaction with participation in various daily activities.

No study has been conducted on the relationship between sensory processing and executive function and their impacts on participation in adults with ADHD. A few studies with a similar approach have been conducted in other populations. Research indicates that executive dysfunctions may exacerbate the negative impacts of adverse sensory processing on daily activity performance, particularly in older adults (28), and difficulties of adults with specific learning disabilities in using executive function abilities efficiently might be tied to a high sensory threshold and passive self-regulation strategies (30). The results of the linear regression analysis in our study indicated that both executive function and sensory processing significantly predict participation in meaningful activities among adults with ADHD. The study’s findings, which revealed a coefficient of 27.7 for the predictive impact of executive function and processing, highlight the need to investigate additional factors influencing participation within this population. This emphasizes the necessity for further research in this area, but given the observed correlation between sensory processing and participation in this population, the findings suggest that addressing these challenges and implementing suitable interventions, when needed, could enhance the participation of adults with ADHD.

Our study is one of the first to explore the relationship between sensory processing problems and executive function in individuals with ADHD; however, it has some limitations. First, the assessment test items could have been more pleasing for most participants. Adults with ADHD often struggle with tolerance to prolonged tasks. Secondly, recruiting adults with ADHD proved challenging, as they frequently do not seek treatment in non-acute situations. The findings highlight differences in executive functioning, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities between adults with and without ADHD, suggesting a possible association among these, although further research is needed.

5.1. Conclusions

The results of the present study indicated that individuals with ADHD have significant differences in executive function, sensory processing, and participation in meaningful activities compared to their healthy peers. Additionally, the correlation between executive function and sensory processing and participation highlights the importance of these factors in individuals’ involvement in meaningful activities. The predictive effect of executive function and sensory processing on participation in meaningful activities also highlights the importance of cognitive and sensory skills in enhancing the quality of life for individuals.

Acknowledgments

Footnotes

References

  • 1.
    Scahill L, Schwab-Stone M. Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2000;9(3):541-55. vii. [PubMed ID: 10944656].
  • 2.
    Faraone SV, Bellgrove MA, Brikell I, Cortese S, Hartman CA, Hollis C, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Rev Dis Primers. 2024;10(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41572-024-00495-0.
  • 3.
    Svenaeus F. Diagnosing mental disorders and saving the normal. Med, Health Care Phil. 2013;17(2):241-4. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11019-013-9529-6.
  • 4.
    Holst Y, Thorell LB. Functional impairments among adults with ADHD: A comparison with adults with other psychiatric disorders and links to executive deficits. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2020;27(3):243-55. [PubMed ID: 30624968]. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1532429.
  • 5.
    Guo N, Fuermaier ABM, Koerts J, Mueller BW, Diers K, Mross A, et al. Neuropsychological functioning of individuals at clinical evaluation of adult ADHD. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2021;128(7):877-91. [PubMed ID: 33355692]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC8295106]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-020-02281-0.
  • 6.
    Pennington BF, Ozonoff S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37(1):51-87. [PubMed ID: 8655658]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x.
  • 7.
    Rosello B, Berenguer C, Baixauli I, Mira A, Martinez-Raga J, Miranda A. Empirical examination of executive functioning, ADHD associated behaviors, and functional impairments in adults with persistent ADHD, remittent ADHD, and without ADHD. BMC Psychiatry. 2020;20(1):134. [PubMed ID: 32204708]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7092442]. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02542-y.
  • 8.
    Smith MC. Sensory Integration: Theory and Practice. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: F.A. Davis Company; 2019.
  • 9.
    Costa-Lopez B, Ferrer-Cascales R, Ruiz-Robledillo N, Albaladejo-Blazquez N, Baryla-Matejczuk M. Relationship between Sensory Processing and Quality of Life: A Systematic Review. J Clin Med. 2021;10(17). [PubMed ID: 34501408]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC8432132]. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10173961.
  • 10.
    Schulze M, Lux. S. A. Sensory Processing in Adult ADHD – A Systematic Review. Res Square. 2020;Preprint. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-71514/v1.
  • 11.
    Kamath MS, Dahm CR, Tucker JR, Huang-Pollock CL, Etter NM, Neely KA. Sensory profiles in adults with and without ADHD. Res Dev Disabil. 2020;104:103696. [PubMed ID: 32526674]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7786815]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103696.
  • 12.
    Sjöwall D, Thorell LB. Neuropsychological deficits in relation to ADHD symptoms, quality of life, and daily life functioning in young adulthood. Appl Neuropsychol. 2022;29(1):32-40. [PubMed ID: 31881160]. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2019.1704287.
  • 13.
    Stern A, Pollak Y, Bonne O, Malik E, Maeir A. The Relationship Between Executive Functions and Quality of Life in Adults With ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2017;21(4):323-30. [PubMed ID: 24189201]. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054713504133.
  • 14.
    Roth RM, Isquith PK, Gioia GA. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function®--Adult Version. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2005. Available from: https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft86244-000.
  • 15.
    Mani A, Ghelijkhani S, Haghighat R, Ahmadzadeh L, Chohedri E, Heydari ST. Validity and Reliability of the Persian Version of the Self-Report Form of Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult version (BRIEF-A). Shiraz E-Med J. 2018;In Press(In Press). https://doi.org/10.5812/semj.14295.
  • 16.
    Brown C. Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile. In: Brown C, editor. Assessments in Occupational Therapy Mental Health. Oxfordshire, England: Routledge; 2024. p. 445-56. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003522645-33.
  • 17.
    Zaree M, Hassani Mehraban A, Lajevardi L, Saneii S, Pashazadeh Azari Z, Mohammadian Rasnani F. Translation, reliability and validity of Persian version of Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile in dementia. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2023;30(1):1-7. [PubMed ID: 33874830]. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2021.1904927.
  • 18.
    Eakman AM. Measurement characteristics of the engagement in meaningful activities survey in an age-diverse sample. Am J Occup Ther. 2012;66(2):e20-9. [PubMed ID: 22394537]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3298038]. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2012.001867.
  • 19.
    Cheraghifard M, Akbarfahimi M, Azad A, Eakman AM, Taghizadeh G. Validation of the Persian Version of the Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey (EMAS) in an Iranian Stroke Population: Predictors of Participation in Meaningful Activities. Am J Occup Ther. 2022;76(4). [PubMed ID: 35771732]. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2022.046623.
  • 20.
    Sun J, Buys N. Early executive function deficit in preterm children and its association with neurodevelopmental disorders in childhood: a literature review. Int J Adolesc Med Health. 2012;24(4):291-9. [PubMed ID: 23183728]. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijamh.2012.042.
  • 21.
    Silverstein MJ, Faraone SV, Leon TL, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Adler LA. The Relationship Between Executive Function Deficits and DSM-5-Defined ADHD Symptoms. J Atten Disord. 2020;24(1):41-51. [PubMed ID: 30296883]. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718804347.
  • 22.
    Fogel Y, Rosenblum S, Hirsh R, Chevignard M, Josman N. Daily Performance of Adolescents with Executive Function Deficits: An Empirical Study Using a Complex-Cooking Task. Occup Ther Int. 2020;2020:3051809. [PubMed ID: 32099539]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7026736]. https://doi.org/10.1155/2020/3051809.
  • 23.
    Toh WX, Yang H, Hartanto A. Executive Function and Subjective Well-being in Middle and Late Adulthood. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2020;75(6):e69-77. [PubMed ID: 30657966]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7457179]. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz006.
  • 24.
    Zhang SY, Qiu SW, Pan MR, Zhao MJ, Zhao RJ, Liu L, et al. Adult ADHD, executive function, depressive/anxiety symptoms, and quality of life: A serial two-mediator model. J Affect Disord. 2021;293:97-108. [PubMed ID: 34175595]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.06.020.
  • 25.
    Fabio RA, Orsino C, Lecciso F, Levante A, Suriano R. Atypical sensory processing in adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A comparative study. Res Dev Disabil. 2024;146:104674. [PubMed ID: 38306842]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2024.104674.
  • 26.
    Andersson H, Sutton D, Bejerholm U, Argentzell E. Experiences of sensory input in daily occupations for people with serious mental illness. Scand J Occup Ther. 2021;28(6):446-56. [PubMed ID: 32544360]. https://doi.org/10.1080/11038128.2020.1778784.
  • 27.
    MacLean J, Finkelstein SA, Paredes-Echeverri S, Perez DL, Ranford J. Sensory Processing Difficulties in Patients with Functional Neurological Disorder: Occupational Therapy Management Strategies and Two Cases. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2022;41:100951. [PubMed ID: 35450672]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spen.2022.100951.
  • 28.
    Engel-Yeger B, Rosenblum S. Executive dysfunctions mediate between altered sensory processing and daily activity performance in older adults. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21(1):132. [PubMed ID: 33618664]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC7898742]. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-021-02032-0.
  • 29.
    Thorell LB, Holst Y, Sjowall D. Quality of life in older adults with ADHD: links to ADHD symptom levels and executive functioning deficits. Nord J Psychiatry. 2019;73(7):409-16. [PubMed ID: 31380715]. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039488.2019.1646804.
  • 30.
    Sharfi K, Rosenblum S, Meyer S. Relationships between executive functions and sensory patterns among adults with specific learning disabilities as reflected in their daily functioning. PLoS One. 2022;17(4). e0266385. [PubMed ID: 35390062]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC8989333]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0266385.
comments

Leave a comment here


Crossmark
Crossmark
Checking
Share on
Cited by
Metrics

Purchasing Reprints

  • Copyright Clearance Center (CCC) handles bulk orders for article reprints for Brieflands. To place an order for reprints, please click here (   https://www.copyright.com/landing/reprintsinquiryform/ ). Clicking this link will bring you to a CCC request form where you can provide the details of your order. Once complete, please click the ‘Submit Request’ button and CCC’s Reprints Services team will generate a quote for your review.
Search Relations

Author(s):

Related Articles