Impact of Long-Term Use of Methylphenidate on Visual Memory of Drug-Naïve Children with Attention Deficit Disorder

authors:

avatar Roheila Seyedtabaei 1 , avatar Reza Seyedtabaei 2 , avatar Seyed Davood Mohammadi 3 , * , avatar Mehdi Tehranidust 4

Department of Microbiology, Immunology and Emory Vaccine Center, Emory University, Atlanta, United States
Department of Psychiatry, Qom University of Medical Sciences, Qom, Iran
Department of Psychiatry, Fasa University of Medical Sciences, Fasa, Iran
Department of Psychiatry, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran

How To Cite Seyedtabaei R, Seyedtabaei R, Mohammadi S D, Tehranidust M. Impact of Long-Term Use of Methylphenidate on Visual Memory of Drug-Naïve Children with Attention Deficit Disorder. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. 2018;12(4):e7899. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs.7899.

Abstract

Background:

Diverse cognitive functions and behaviors have been monitored in the two sub-types of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) including the combined type and the inattentive type.

Objectives:

Previous studies have shown that ADHD children have problems in visual memory, and short and long-term use of methylphenidate (MPH) improves these functions, but fewer studies have been done on the inattentive subgroup, namely attention deficit disorder (ADD). Due to the different cognitive functions in these two ADHD subgroups, this study was done to investigate the long-term use of MPH on the visual memory of ADD children.

Methods:

A 4-week experimental clinical trial using MPH (1 mg/kg/dose) was conducted. Participants were 20 children aged 6 - 11 years with ADD that came to the Rouzbeh Clinic in Tehran in 2010. Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB) tests of visual memory were used for assessment.

Results:

The long-term use of MPH improved 12 aspects of paired associated learning (PAL) such as first-trial memory score, the number of mean mistakes to success and mean efforts to success (P < 0.05). However, MPH did not improve the stages completed in the first trial, the total errors, and the total errors adjusted in the three-shape step of PAL (P > 0.05). MPH also improved all aspects of pattern recognition memory (PRM) (P < 0.05) and the mean correct latency of spatial recognition memory (SRM) (P < 0.05). However, MPH had no effect on delayed matching to sample (DMS) (P > 0.05).

Conclusions:

MPH improved performance on the PAL, PRM, and SRM visual evaluating tests of ADD patients. Nevertheless, the patients did not show any improvement in the DMS test. In comparison with previous studies, our results would suggest that MPH has similar effects on the visual memory of ADD and ADHD patients.

1. Background

The two sub-types of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, including combined inattentive and hyperactivity disorder or ADHD and the permanently inattentive disorder or ADD, have different cognitive functions (1-4).

Individuals with ADHD show defects in inhibitory control and planning whereas problems in set-shifting, vigilance, and interference control can be more related to ADD (5). Underactivity and apathy are associated more with ADD patients than with ADHD patients (6). ADD patients are commonly recognized by sluggish cognitive tempo, but ADHD individuals are more hyperactive.

Different parts of the brain are affected in ADHD and ADD individuals. In ADHD patients, the frontostriatal region is affected. The defect in the striatal part of the brain results in hyperactivity (1, 7). In ADD patients, the frontoparietal circuit is affected. This causes weakness in language learning, mathematics, working memory, and reactions to stimuli (1, 8).

Stimulant therapy by methylphenidate (MPH) is the common medication of ADHD. It prevents the reuptake of dopamine and norepinephrine transporters in the presynaptic areas of this neurotransmitter (9). It not only improves the concentration and decreases the hyperactivity behavior of the patients, but also improves their cognitive function. Functional neuroimaging gives rise to this proposition that MPH regulates the function of the brain by arising frontal and reducing striatal stimulation in normal individuals (10).

Visual memory is necessary for having good working memory and learning. Children with a deficit in visual working memory have some problems with recalling the visual appearance of words and letters, so having difficulty with reading and writing.

ADHD patients have shown difficulty in CANTAB tests of visual memory, such as paired associated learning (PAL), pattern recognition memory (PRM), spatial recognition memory (SRM), and delayed matching to sample (DMS) (11-14). Coghill et al. and Rhodes et al. have shown that the chronic use of MPH improves ADHD patients’ performance in these tests (12, 15). The effect of acute MPH on visual memory tests is controversy as some studies showed improvements on DMS, PRM, and SRM tests (10-12, 16) and a few showed no improvement on PRM and SRM (17).

MPH is also used in ADD patients. Nevertheless, ADD patients are presented with different symptoms in comparison with ADHD individuals. Thus, little effort has been made to show the effect of MPH on different aspects of these patients’ behavior.

This paper studied the effect of MPH on the visual memory of ADD patients. As we will show, our empirical results suggest that MPH improves the visual memory of individuals with ADD.

2. Objectives

Previous studies have shown that ADHD children have problems in visual memory, and short and long-term use of MPH improves these functions, but fewer studies have been done on the inattentive subgroup, namely ADD. Due to the different cognitive functions in these two ADHD subgroups, this study was done to investigate the long-term use of MPH on the visual memory of ADD children.

3. Materials and Methods

This study obtained an ethical approval. All participants, parents, or guardians provided their written informed consent. Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB) visual memory tests were taken from ADD patients.

3.1. Instrument

3.1.1. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery

The best-available computerized test battery for the visual memory is CANTAB (14). It benefits from an extensive bibliography of over 1600 peer-reviewed papers, more than in any other computerized cognitive tests, ensuring the most validated measurement available. The high levels of construct validity increase the likelihood that CANTAB outcomes are clinically meaningful. All CANTAB tests have satisfactory levels of test-retest reliability, with some outcome measures reaching test-retest correlations of around 0.9 (18). CANTAB has four main visual memory tests: PAL, PRM, SRM, and DMS.

3.1.2. Paired Associates Learning

In the PAL test, pattern-based boxes are randomly opened on the screen. The participants must find and palpate the box in the site in which the pattern originally was located. For the test to become gradually more challenging, the number of patterns increases from one to eight. This test evaluates the visual memory and the new learning ability of the participants.

PAL result scales cover the errors made by the participant, the number of efforts, the memory points, and the completed stages.

3.1.3. Delayed Matching to Sample

In DMS, visual patterns are displayed on the screen for a few seconds and after a short pause, four similar patterns will appear. Participants need to recognize and select the most similar models to the original one. This test evaluates the short-term visual memory of the patient. DMS is particularly useful for recognizing defects in the medial temporal lobe area. The outcome measures of the test cover the number of correct and wrong attempts.

3.1.4. Pattern Recognition Memory

In the PRM test, a series of 12 visual patterns are presented to the participants first. In the recognition phase, a two-alternative forced choice is presented. The choices are shown in the opposite order of original patterns. The participants are asked to select the pattern they have already seen. This test evaluates the visual model recognition memory of the patients in a double-alternative forced choice model. The outcome measures of the test include the number of correct efforts and the latent time (the speed of participants to respond).

3.1.5. Spatial Recognition Memory

In SRM, a white square is shown at five diverse sites on the screen. In the recognition step, the participants are asked to locate the square presented in the presentation phase among a series of five pairs of squares. As with the PRM test, the location of the squares is presented in the reverse order of the presentation. PRM and SRM comprise different parts of PAL and all evaluate the visual memory.

This test has three outcome points, including the number and the percentage of correct efforts, and the latent time.

3.2. Participants

In this study, we evaluated 20 children (aged 6 to 11), who were referred from the Rouzbeh Clinic in Tehran in 2010 to the Institute for Cognitive Science Studies (ICSS). Exclusion criteria were having a learning disorder or mental retardation (IQ less than 80), having a chronic bodily illness, sensory or motor deficiency, current or previous taking of stimulant drug, using any illegal drugs, the existence of popular comorbid conditions, neurological deficiency, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and anxiety disorder.

Eligible children had DSM-IV criteria for ADD and had an interview with an expert child and adolescent psychiatrist (Dr. Tehranidoust). Guardians answered the ADD rating scale and the Conners’ parent rating scale and completed a demographic form. Then, the IQ of the children was evaluated by the WISC-V test.

3.3. Design

The participants were first evaluated by taking the CANTAB and Conners tests. Then, they were treated with MPH 0.5 mg/kg twice a day for 4 weeks. After finishing the treatment process, the participants were re-evaluated by taking the CANTAB and Conners tests. The CANTAB test results were gathered for further evaluation.

3.4. Statistical Analysis

SPSS (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois) program was used to analyze the data. Paired t-test was used to evaluate the effect of MPH.

4. Results

4.1. Clinical Response to MPH Using Connors and ADHD Rating Scale Tests

According to Table 1 that illustrates the results of the Conners and ADHD Rating Scale tests, ADD children had significant improvements in inattention, hyperactivity, and ADHD indices of the Conners Rating Scale, after receiving MPH for a month. However, the oppositional index had an insignificant improvement.

Table 1.

The Effect of Long-Term Use of MPH on Connors-RS and ADHD-RSa

TestsWeek 0Week 4P Value
CPRS
Oppositional index56.80 (9.0)58.05 (8.6)0.46
Inattention index67.45 (9.4)62.55 (11.0)0.03*
Hyperactivity index59.30 (9.4)55.10 (7.7)0.02*
ADHD index64.85 (8.9)59.80 (9.4)0.008*
ADHD RS
Inattention86.20 (19.1)79.30 (21.1)0.02*
Hyperactivity76.35 (12.4)69.40 (22.3)0.21
Total score83.95 (16.6)76.10 (21.2)0.02*

ADD children also showed significant improvements in the inattention aspect and the total score of the ADHD Rating Scale but they did not have meaningful improvements in terms of hyperactivity.

4.2. The Results of the CANTAB Test

4.2.1. Paired Associates Learning

The chronic use of MPH significantly improved the performance of ADD patients in the first trial memory score, mean errors to success, mean efforts to success, total errors, total errors adjusted, total errors with six shapes, total errors with eight shapes, total errors with six shapes adjusted, total errors with eight shapes adjusted, total trials, and total trials adjusted. However, it did not improve the performance of ADD patients in the number of stages completed in the first trial, total errors with three shapes, and total errors with three shapes adjusted.

4.2.2. Pattern Recognition Memory

The ADD patients showed significant improvements in the mean correct latency, the number and percentage of correct efforts as the indices of PRM tests, after a chronic use of MPH.

4.2.3. Spatial Recognition Memory

MPH only improved the performance of the ADD patients in the mean correct latency index of SRM tests. The patients had no significant improvements in the number and percentage of correct efforts.

Table 2.

Effect of the Long-Term Use of MPH on PAL, PRM, and SRM Test Resultsa

TestsWeek 0Week 4TP Value
PAL
First trial memory score11.75 (3.3)15.40 (2.9)-5.87< 0.001*
Mean errors to success4.37 (3.3)2.26 (2.2)3.940.001*
Mean trials to success2.30 (0.78)1.87 (0.63)3.030.007*
Stages completed5.00 (0.00)5.00 (0.00)
Stages completed in the first trial2.50 (0.82)3.00 (0.97)-2.510.02*
Total errors21.8 (16.6)11.30 (11.0)3.940.001*
Total errors adjusted21.8 (16.6)11.30 (11.0)3.940.001*
Total errors three shapes1.35 (2.2)0.50 (0.88)1.540.138
Total errors six shapes7.25 (11.2)2.90 (5.4)2.100.04*
Total errors eight shapes12.45 (7.8)7.45 (5.6)3.210.005*
Total errors three shapes adjusted1.35 (2.2)0.50 (0.88)1.540.138
Total errors six shapes adjusted7.25 (11.2)2.90 (5.4)2.100.048*
Total errors eight shapes adjusted12.45 (7.8)7.45 (5.6)3.030.005*
Total trials11.5 (3.9)9.35 (3.1)3.030.007*
Total trials adjusted11.50 (3.9)9.35 (3.1)3.210.007*
PRM
Mean correct latency3145.92 (1018.1)2266.21 (468.3)5.21< 0.001*
Number of correct efforts19.50 (3.4)21.70 (2.2)-4.26< 0.001*
Percentage of correct efforts81.24 (14.5)90.41 (9.5)-4.26< 0.001*
SRM
Mean correct latency3198.58 (778.3)2791.78 (651.9)2.290.03*
Number of correct efforts13.15 (2.2)13.65 (2.4)-1.120.27
Percentage of correct efforts65.75 (11.3)68.25 (12.3)-1.120.27

4.2.4. Delayed Matching to Sample

MPH had no significant effect on the indices of DMS tests such as DMS A, DMS B, the mean correct latent time, the mean correct latent time all delays, the mean accuracy simultaneously, the mean correct latency (0, 4000 and 12000 msec), the percentage of corrects, the percentage of corrects all delays, the percentage of corrects simultaneously, the percentage of corrects (0, 4000 and 12000 msec), prob error given correct, prob error given error, total corrects, total corrects all delays, total corrects simultaneously, and total corrects (0, 4000 and 12000 msec) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Effect of Long-Term Use of MPH on DMS Testa

DMS TestWeek 0Week 4TP Value
DMS A 41 (0.31)0.44 (0.10)-2.140.16
DMS B -0.63 (0.46)-0.59 (0.42)-0.360.71
Mean correct latency4344.69 (1006.3)4717.65 (1126.6)-1.340.19
Mean correct latency all delays4354.60 (1165.4)4695.01 (1536.3)-0.800.43
Mean corrects simultaneously4525.62 (1208.9)4522.20 (1130.0)0.010.98
Mean correct latency 0 sec3693.25 (1178.7)4274.50 (1687.2)-1.250.22
Mean correct latency 4 sec4108.82 (1391.5)4359.68 (1644.5)-0.490.62
Mean correct latency 12 sec4979.06 (1400.8)5788.41 (2629.0)-1.290.21
Percent of corrects76.00 (13.1)77.25 (14.1)-0.310.75
Percent of corrects all delays68.96 (16.3)72.33 (18.8)-0.750.46
Percent of corrects simultaneously97.00 (7.3)92.00 (16.4)1.150.26
Percent corrects 0 sec63.00 (24.5)70.00 (23.8)-1.370.18
Percent corrects 4 sec73.00 (22.7)76.00 (27.2)-0.420.67
Percent corrects 12 sec71.00 (22.9)71.00 (22.9)0.001.0
Prob error given correct0.27 (0.16)0.25 (0.15)0.380.70
Prob error given error0.14 (0.18)0.14 (0.17)-0.260.79
Total corrects15.20 (2.6)15.45 (2.8)-0.310.75
Total corrects all delays10.35 (2.4)10.83 (2.8)-0.740.46
Total corrects simultaneously4.85 (.36)4.60 (0.82)1.150.26
Total corrects 0 sec3.15 (1.2)3.50 (1.1)0.000.18
Total corrects 4 sec3.65 (1.1)3.80 (1.3)-0.420.67
Total corrects 12 sec3.55 (1.1)3.55 (1.1)-1.371.0

5. Discussion

The goal of this research was to assess the influence of MPH on the visual memory of ADD patients. Four main CANTAB tests (PAL, SRM, PRM, and DMS) were used.

Our experimental results illustrated that MPH significantly enhanced the performance of the ADD patients in PAL, SRM, and PRM tests. However, the patients did not have significant improvements in DMS tests.

ADD patients suffer from learning problems. MPH is used for the treatment of both subtypes, but a few studies have been done to show its effect on ADD patients. Different characteristics are seen in these two subtypes as ADHD individuals are known more hyperactive and ADD patients more inattentive and underactive. We believe that MPH shows different effects on brain functions of these two subtypes, and it may even have negative effects on some performances. We conducted this study to evidence more on the underlying cause of learning defects in ADD individuals, and show how MPH affects it.

Visual memory is the core need for learning and writing. The center of the brain involved in the recalling visual memory is the posterior parietal region. Studies have proven that ADHD patients have visual memory problems (11-14). Nevertheless, the part of the brain that is defected in these individuals is the frontostriatal regions, rather than the posterior-parietal. This could lead to the result that ADHD patient’s problems in visual memory are due to their lack of inhibitory control than a defect in the visual memory region. MPH by regulating the work of the striatal region improves their control on visual memory tests (12, 15).

MPH improves visual memory in ADD patients not by modulating the striatal region but by acting on the visual memory center in the parietal region. ADD patients have defects in the parietal-frontal circuit, and MPH enhances the activity of this region.

A limitation of our study was finding ADD children between six and eleven years old, without previous medication. Another limitation of this research was the side effects of MPH such as palpitation and anorexia that made some patients to stop the medication or to change the prescribed doses. More research should be conducted on ADD patients to evaluate the visual memory of these patients.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Diamond A. Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): a neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity). Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17(3):807-25. [PubMed ID: 16262993]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC1474811]. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050388.

  • 2.

    Lahey BB, Carlson CL. Validity of the diagnostic category of attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity: A review of the literature. J Learn Disabil. 1991;24(2):110-20. [PubMed ID: 2010673]. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221949102400208.

  • 3.

    Riccio CA, Homack S, Jarratt KP, Wolfe ME. Differences in academic and executive function domains among children with ADHD Predominantly Inattentive and Combined Types. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21(7):657-67. [PubMed ID: 16920328]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.05.010.

  • 4.

    Kordon A, Kahl KG, Wahl K. A new understanding of attention-deficit disorders--beyond the age-at-onset criterion of DSM-IV. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256 Suppl 1:i47-54. [PubMed ID: 16977552]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-006-1007-1.

  • 5.

    Nigg JT, Blaskey LG, Huang-Pollock CL, Rappley MD. Neuropsychological executive functions and DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(1):59-66. [PubMed ID: 11800208]. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200201000-00012.

  • 6.

    Bauermeister JJ, Matos M, Reina G, Salas CC, Martinez JV, Cumba E, et al. Comparison of the DSM-IV combined and inattentive types of ADHD in a school-based sample of Latino/Hispanic children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(2):166-79. [PubMed ID: 15679525]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00343.x.

  • 7.

    Hale TS, Hariri AR, McCracken JT. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: perspectives from neuroimaging. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2000;6(3):214-9. [PubMed ID: 10982499]. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:3<214::AID-MRDD9>3.0.CO;2-M.

  • 8.

    Peers PV, Ludwig CJ, Rorden C, Cusack R, Bonfiglioli C, Bundesen C, et al. Attentional functions of parietal and frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(10):1469-84. [PubMed ID: 15689522]. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi029.

  • 9.

    Pliszka SR. The neuropsychopharmacology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1385-90. [PubMed ID: 15950012]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.026.

  • 10.

    Mehta MA, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Mavaddat N, Pickard JD, Robbins TW. Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete frontal and parietal lobe regions in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2000;20(6):RC65. [PubMed ID: 10704519].

  • 11.

    Kempton S, Vance A, Maruff P, Luk E, Costin J, Pantelis C. Executive function and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: stimulant medication and better executive function performance in children. Psychol Med. 1999;29(3):527-38. [PubMed ID: 10405075].

  • 12.

    Rhodes SM, Coghill DR, Matthews K. Methylphenidate restores visual memory, but not working memory function in attention deficit-hyperkinetic disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;175(3):319-30. [PubMed ID: 15138760]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-004-1833-7.

  • 13.

    Douglas VI. Cognitive control processes in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In: Quay HC, Hogan AE, editors. Handbook of disruptive behavior disorders. Boston, MA: Springer; 1999. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-4881-2.

  • 14.

    Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336-46. [PubMed ID: 15950006]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006.

  • 15.

    Coghill DR, Rhodes SM, Matthews K. The neuropsychological effects of chronic methylphenidate on drug-naive boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(9):954-62. [PubMed ID: 17543895]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.030.

  • 16.

    Rhodes SM, Coghill DR, Matthews K. Acute neuropsychological effects of methylphenidate in stimulant drug-naive boys with ADHD II--broader executive and non-executive domains. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(11):1184-94. [PubMed ID: 17076758]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01633.x.

  • 17.

    Mehta MA, Goodyer IM, Sahakian BJ. Methylphenidate improves working memory and set-shifting in AD/HD: relationships to baseline memory capacity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(2):293-305. [PubMed ID: 14982243].

  • 18.

    Cambridge Cognition. CANTAB® [Cognitive assessment software]. 2016. Available from: http://www.cambridgecognition.com/cantab-faqs#sthash.uOCzX994.WDJUglRz.dpuf.