Abstract
Keywords
Introduction
Stress is considered as an important factor which can influence embryos growth during the gestation period of life. Stress has been defined as any condition that changes internal or external melio. Stress reaction differs in accordance with its severity to threats in life (1, 2). In general, it is accepted that stress can induce a neuroendocrine response originated from hypothalamus and ends in adrenal gland (3). Corticosterone and norepinephrine released from rodents’ adrenal cortex and medulla respectively, prepare the animals for ameliorate the menace and/or overcome it (3). However, when a pregnant animal is exposed to a stressful event, with a possible increase in the plasma concentration of its corticosterone, the overloaded hormone may readily cross the placenta barrier and affect its embryos growth and development (4, 5). In this regard, studies in rodents indicated that the prenatal stress increased the anxiety-related behaviors (6). In addition, prenatal stress may be linked to a typical laterality in rats (7-9). Investigators also revealed that the early life stress can alter dopaminergic system activity in rats (4), which may influence their response to the psycho stimulants, indicating the importance of early life events on later brain activities. All together, these findings indicated that implication of stress to a pregnant animal could lead to a serious abnormal brain function of its off-springs. However, despite the intense investigations, regarding to the effects of prenatal stress on the function and morphology of the brain dopaminergic system (4, 9, 10) and the effects of postnatal stress on the dopaminergic (9) and opioidergic system (11, 12), there is no study indicating the role of a maternal mild stress on the response of the off-springs to morphine-induced locomotion in mice. Such study may provide more information to reach a better understanding of what may occur in brain’s opioidergic system after the prenatal stress.
Experimental
Animals
Thirty-two male and female NMRI mice (Pasture Institute, Tehran, Iran) were housed one by one in a mating cage (15×15×20 cm) over the night for mating. At 8:0.0 PM the following day, the animals were separated and the Embryonic Zero day (E0) was detected through observing the vaginal plug and/or identifying the sperm in vaginal smear. The pregnant mice were randomly divided into control and experimental groups (n = 12/group). The animals were housed 2/cage and remained in a standard animal room conditions (22 ± 2°C and 50% humidity with 12 h light/dark cycle) and have free access to standard mouse chow and tap water add lib except during the experiments.
Experimental procedure: Stress protocol in pregnant mice (i.e. F1 generation)
All experiments took place in light time (10:00-12:00). To induct the stress, each animal was placed in a PVC cylinder (20 cm length, 6.5 cm diameter) in which one of the ends was blocked (13, 14) and the other one was sealed by pieces of plastic. In such apparatus, animals could not move easily though their abdomens were not under the pressure. The pregnant mice were placed in these vacuums 1 h/day for the next 15 days (Embryonic day 15) (13, 14). The time of stress exposure was randomly designed for minimum stress adaptations. On E16, the stress was interrupted and animals were kept for delivering their pups and nursing them.
Blood sampling and plasma corticosterone levels detection in stressed pregnant mice
Blood samples were taken from retro-orbital sinus (0.1 mL of the blood in 0.9 mL sodium citrate 5%) of the pregnant mice on Embryonic day 8 (E8) of pregnancy between 10:00-12:00 (13, 14). The samples were centrifuged in 3000 rpm for 5 min in 4°C and the supper natant serum was collected for corticosterone detection. Corticosterone concentration was determined by ELISA kit (Rat Corticosterone ELISA kit; EIA-4164; DRG Instruments GmbH, Germany) in 450 nm.
Determination of left-handedness and right-handedness in F2 generation When the off-springs’ (both males and females) weight reached up to 20 g, the animals were placed in a T-maze apparatus and their tendency for left or right arm of the maze was estimated (n = 30 for each session/sex; 4 groups with total number of 120 mice). All experiments were taken place in a silent room from 10:0.0 AM to 17:0.0 PM. The experiment was repeated three times for each animal and the animal’s arm which was selected more than once (i.e., 2 of 3), was considered as its tendency. The experiments took place in separate rooms for males and females.
Locomotion activity in F2 generation
After right-handedness and left-handedness determination, the next generations (e.g. F2) were engaged in locomotor activity experiments. All experiments took place in a silent room from 10:0.0 AM till 17:0.0 PM (15). Male and female F2 generation of the experiment or control mothers (n = 7-8/group, total number = 131 for males and 140 for females) were brought to the lab and remain there 1 h for adaptation. These animals were different from the animals used in the left-handedness and right-handedness determination. Each animal was placed in an open field apparatus (a cylinder with 30 cm diameter and 30 cm high made by Plexiglas), and after 5 min, its locomotion activity was recorded for 10 min by a digital camera. These experiments also took place in separate rooms for male and female mice.
Drugs
Morphine sulfate (TEMAD Co., Tehran, Iran), ± sulpiride ([±]-N-1-[Ethylpyrrolidin-2-ylmethyl])-2-methoxy-5-sulfamoylbenzamide), (Sigma Chemical Co., California, USA) and dextromethorphan hydrobromide (TOCRIS, UK) were dissolved in normal saline (10 mL) and were injected in volume of 0.1 mL/10 g per body weight. Morphine was injected subcutaneously (s.c.). Sulpiride and dextromethorphan were injected intra-peritoneally (IP) 30 min before the beginning of experiments. The doses for all drugs were chosen according to the previous studies (15-17).
Statistical analysis
Data are shown as mean ± SEM. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey post-test, Un-Paired t-test or chi-square tests was applied. p < 0.05 considered statistically significant.
Results and Discussion
Effects of stress on plasma corticosterone level in pregnant mice
The effect of stress on blood corticosterone level in pregnant animals is shown in Figure 1. Our results indicated that milled stress can be efficient for the elevation of corticosterone level in the experimental group for about 300%, which was statistically significant (p < 0.01, Figure 1).
Determination of right or left-handedness in off-springs
When the off-springs of the control group were placed in T-maze, more than 80% of the males and females preferred the right arm . However, there sults of off-springs in experimental mothers were completely reversed as up to 70% of both males and females preferred their left arm to the right one (p < 0.001, Figure 2).
Exploratory behavior in non-treated F2 generation mice
Animals which did not receive a treatment were located in an open field apparatus and their activity was measured for 10 min. Results showed that the experimental groups were more active than the control one (p < 0.05, Figure 3)
Morphine induced locomotion in off-springs
As shown in Figures 4A-4D, the administration of different doses of morphine (0.5, 5, and 50 mg/Kg) induced locomotor activity in the off-springs of control mothers (Figure 4A for males and Figure 4B for females). Moreover, maximum response was achieved in dose of 50 mg/Kg of morphine in both animals. In addition, it seems that females were more responsible to morphine than males (Figures 4A and 4B, p < 0.001).
On the other hand, our results for the off-springs of experimental mothers revealed that morphine induces hypoactivity in doses 0.5 and 5 mg/Kg in both sexes (Figures 4C and 4D, p < 0.0001). Interestingly, the females of the experimental group also showed hypo activity for dose of 50 mg/Kg of morphine instead of hyper activity induced by the same dose in males (Figures 4C and 4D).
By injecting different doses of dopamine D2 receptor antagonist, sulpiride (25 and 50 mg/Kg, SC) induced hypoactivity in all animals (Figures 4A, 4B, 4C, and 4D, p < 0.001). However, the N- methyl-D- aspartate (NMDA) glutamate receptor antagonist, dextromethorphan, (20 mg/Kg, IP) did not change the animals’ locomotor activity in all groups.
First of all, we found that psychological stress (i.e. restraint in a closed tube) can increase plasma corticosterone level in the pregnant mice. Second, the study revealed that off-springs from stressed mothers had different tendency for left or right preference, and third, off-spring’s responsibility to morphine was different according to their sex.
Our results showed that corticosterone plasma level was elevated in the experimental group which indicated the method effectiveness for stress induction. It is now accepted that incrementing in plasma corticosterone level is an indicator of stress response in rodents (3, 18, 19), which further indicates the involvement of HPA axis activity as the main stress response element (2, 20). In agreement with our results, it is clear that immobilization of male rats also increases plasma corticosterone level (13, 14, 21). Our results further indicated that the milled restraint stress in the female pregnant mice canincrease the plasma corticosterone level as well.
Previous studies have indicated that intra-uterine stress can affect the brain laterality (22-24) and may be linked to drug abuse (11, 12, 25). Our results showed that left-handedness and right-handedness was equal in both males and females in the control group whereas it was a shift to left-handedness in both males and females in the experimental group. It seems that female mice are more sensitive to intra-uterine stress, in comparison with the males. However, the exact reason for the observed results is not available and needs further experiments. These results further indicated that a milled restraint stress which can increase plasma corticosterone level in the pregnant mice, can affect the brain laterality in the embryos in the way that the F2 generation showed the complete diversity from the controls. In a similar line with previous studies, it can be concluded that some regions within the brain of the experimental F2 generation are not well developed (4, 9, 24, 26-28). Moreover, as it was mentioned earlier, it may also be related to drug abuse. In our experiment, the animal’s response to the new environment was also different in experimental and control groups. Knowing the response of the animals to the new environment is the aim of several studies. For example, it is clear that animals show more activity in the new environment (10), and this is the result of dopamine mesolimbic activity (10). Based on available data, it is important that animals with prenatal stress experiences may suffer from abnormal dopamine mesolimbic system development (4, 9, 28-32). It is likely that corticosterone plasma level increment resulted from restraint stress in the experimental group can interact with factors which influence fetal brain development and the change of locomotion in the new environment is the result.
The response of F2 generation to morphine (as a typical opioid) also showed difference in the experimental group. While the controls showed no response to low doses of morphine, the response of the experimental group was even a decline. In agreement with our results, Michaels et al. showed that the early post-natal stress in rats can change the response to the mu-opioid receptor agonists in place conditioning paradigm (12). In addition, Moffett has shown that post-natal stress can also alter the cocaine self-administration in the rats (33). Alteration in response to morphine may be linked to dopamine mesolimbic system abnormal development and/or opioid receptor abnormality. It must be mentioned that the morphine’s ability of increasing the locomotion activity is hypothesized to be built base on dopamine mesolimbic activity (34-38). However, as mentioned above, experiments indicated that pre-natal and post-natal stress can impair the development of mesolimbic dopamine system (4, 9). Based on these findings, one may conclude that our experiments, in which the prenatal stress increases the maternal plasma corticosterone level, leads to defect in brain mesolimbic dopamine system development and the response to morphine differs from controls as a result. On the other hand, some contradict data indicated that the response to the opioid agonists was changed in rats experienced early post-natal stress (12), which also may be true for the response observed in the present study.
In completion, our results obtained from D2 dopamine antagonist, sulpiride, may be useful for resolving the above theories. Our results indicated that sulpiride reduced the locomotion activity in all groups. The dopamine D2 receptors have shown to be involved in locomotion and their inhibition leads to hypo activity in both human and animal models (15, 39, 40, 41). Our results are in agreement with these observations and indicate that dopamine D2 receptors are functional in all groups. Considering the results, it concluded that in experimental group, the mesolimbic dopamine system responsiveness to morphine may be different from that of dopamine D2 receptors.
In the last part of our experiments, dextromethorphan as N- methyl-D- aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist (17) was administered to the animals for investigation of possible brain glutamate system involvement and/or alteration in response to intra-uterine stress. However, no response was observed when the drug was administrated. Our results did not regret the role of brain glutamate system in the alteration in response to opioids, even thought other methods such as place conditioning paradigm and self-administration procedure may be useful for further evaluation of the role of this system.
In conclusion, results indicated that first, F2 generation brain laterality was changed in animals whose mothers experienced milled stress. Second, response to morphine-induced locomotion was also altered in the experimental group which was sex dependent but could not be linked to D2 dopamine and NMDA glutamate receptor dysfunction.
Acknowledgements
References
-
1.
Carrasco LD. Neuroendocrine pharmacology of stress. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2003;463:235-272. [PubMed ID: 12600714].
-
2.
Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:434-445. [PubMed ID: 19401723].
-
3.
McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 2007;87:873-904. [PubMed ID: 17615391].
-
4.
Brake WG, Diorio J, Meaney MJ, Gratton A. Influence of early postnatal rearing conditions on mesocorticolimbic dopamine and behavioural responses to psychostimulants and stressors in adult rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004;19:1863-1874. [PubMed ID: 15078560].
-
5.
Cottrell EC, Seck JR. Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids and the programming of adult disease. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;3:1-9. [PubMed ID: 19194528].
-
6.
Mueller BR, Bale TL. Sex-specific programming of offspring emotionality after stress early in pregnancy. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:9055-9065. [PubMed ID: 18768700].
-
7.
Alonso J, Castellano MA, Rodriguez M. Behavioral lateralization in rats: prenatal stress effect on sex differences. Brain Res. 1991;539:45-50. [PubMed ID: 2015502].
-
8.
Alonso J, Navarro E, Santana C, Rodriguez M. Motor lateralization, behavioral despair and dopaminergic brain asymmetry after prenatal stress. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1997;58:443-448. [PubMed ID: 9300604].
-
9.
Weinstock M. Alterations induced by gestational stress in brain morphology and behavior of the off springs. Progr. Neurobiol. 2001;65:427-451.
-
10.
Yanga J, Lia W, Liub X, Lia Z, Lib H, Yangb G, Xua J, Lia J. Enriched environment treatment counteracts enhanced addictive and depressive-like behavior induced by prenatal chronic stress. Brain Res. 2006;3:132-137.
-
11.
Lu L, Hall FS, Shaham Y. Effect of environmental stressors on opiate and psychostimulant reinforcement, reinstatement and discrimination in rats: a review. Neurosc. Biobehav. Rev. 2003;27:457-491.
-
12.
Michaels CC. Early postnatal stress alters place conditioning to both µ- and -opioid agonists. JPET. 2008;325:313-318.
-
13.
Zardooz H, Zahedi Asl S, Gharib Naseri MK, Hedayati M. Effect of chronic restraint stress on carbohydrate metabolism in rat. Physiol. Behav. 2006;89:373-378. [PubMed ID: 16899260].
-
14.
Zardooz H, Zahedi Asl S, Naseri MG. Effect of chronic psychological stress on insulin release from rat isolated pancreatic islets. Life Sci. 2006;79:57-62. [PubMed ID: 16442128].
-
15.
Zarrindast MR, Eliassi A. Differential effects of dopamine agonists on locomotion in intact and reserpine-treated mice. Gen. Pharmacol. 1991;22:1027-31. [PubMed ID: 1687393].
-
16.
Sahraei H, Faghih-Monzavi Z, Fatemi SM, Pashaei-Rad S, Salimi SH, Kamalinejad M. Effects of Papaver rhoeas extract on the acquisition and expression of morphine induced behavioral sensitization in mice. Phytother. Res. 2006;20:737-741. [PubMed ID: 16807881].
-
17.
Lue W, Huang EY, Yang S, Wong C, Tao P. Post-treatment of dextromethorphan reverses morphine effect on conditioned place preference in rats. Synapse. 2007;61:420-428. [PubMed ID: 17372963].
-
18.
Dunn AJ, Swiergiel AH. The role of corticotropin-releasing factor and noradrenaline in stress-related responses, and the inter-relationships between the two systems. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2008;583:186-193. [PubMed ID: 18281033].
-
19.
Lightman SL. The neuroendocrinology of stress: a never ending story. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20:880-884. [PubMed ID: 18601712].
-
20.
McEwen BS. The brain is the central organ of stress and adaptation. Neuroimage. 2009;47:911-913. [PubMed ID: 19501171].
-
21.
Lucas LR, Wang CJ, McCall TJ, McEwen BS. Effects of immobilization stress on neurochemical markers in the motivational system of the male rat. Brain Res. 2008;1155:108-115. [PubMed ID: 17511973].
-
22.
Gutteling BM, Buitelaar JK. Prenatal stress and mixed-handedness. Pediatric Res. 2007;62:586-590.
-
23.
McFarlane A, Bryant RA, Williams LM, Niaura R, Paul RH, Hitsman BL, Stroud L, Alexander DM, Gordon E. The impact of early life stress on psychophysiological, personality and behavioral measures in 740 non-clinical subjects. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2005;3:27-40. [PubMed ID: 16035139].
-
24.
Mulder EJH, Huizink AC, Van den Bergh BRH, Buitelaar JK, Visser GHA. Prenatal maternal stress: effects on pregnancy and the (unborn) child. Early Human Development. 2002;70:3-14. [PubMed ID: 12441200].
-
25.
Mandal MKB, Kumar A, Gupta R. Side-bias in alcohol and heroin addicts. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2000;3:381-383. [PubMed ID: 10906005].
-
26.
Kabbaj M, Watson SJ, Akil H. The search for the neurobiological basis of vulnerability to drug abuse: using microarrays to investigate the role of stress and individual differences. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47:111-122. [PubMed ID: 15464130].
-
27.
Kalinichev M, Holtzman SG. Periodic postpartum separation ration from the offspring results in long-lasting changes in anxiety-related behaviors and sensitivity to morphine in Long-Evans mother rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2000;152:431-439. [PubMed ID: 11140336].
-
28.
Thadani P. The intersection of stress, drug abuse and development. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 2002;27:221-230.
-
29.
Autry AE, Grillo CA, Piroli GG, Rothstein JD, McEwen BS, Reagan LP. Glucocorticoid regulation of GLT-1 glutamate transporter isoform expression in the rat hippocampus. Neuroendocrinol. 2006;83:371-379.
-
30.
Bennur S, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Pawlak R, Strickland S, McEwen BS, Chattarji S. Stress-induced spine loss in the medial amygdala is mediated by tissue-plasminogen activator. Neuroscience. 2007;144:8-16. [PubMed ID: 17049177].
-
31.
Cunningham-Bussel AC, Root JC, Butler T, Tuescher O, Pan H, Epstein J, Weisholtz DS, Pavony M, Silverman ME, Goldstein MS, Altemus M, Cloitre M, Ledoux J, McEwen B, Stern E, Silbersweig D. Diurnal cortisol amplitude and fronto-limbic activity in response to stressful stimuli. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:694-704. [PubMed ID: 19135805].
-
32.
Goldenstein RZV. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimagining evidence for involvement of the frontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2002;159:1624-1652.
-
33.
Moffett MC, Harley J, Francis D, Sanghani SD, Davis WI, Kuhar MJ. Maternal separation and handling affects cocaine self-administration in both the treated pups as adults and the dams. JPET. 2006;3:1210-1218.
-
34.
Acquas E, Di Chiara G. Depression of mesolimbic dopamine transmission and sensitization to morphine during opiate abstinence. J. Neurochem. 1992;58:1620-1625. [PubMed ID: 1313849].
-
35.
Bassareo V, De Luca MA, Di Chiara G. Differential impact of pavlovian drug conditioned stimuli on in-vivodopamine transmission in the rat accumbens shell and core and in the prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacol. 2007;191:689-703.
-
36.
Di Chiara G, Imperato A. Preferential stimulation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens by opiates, alcohol, and barbiturates: studies with transcerebral dialysis in freely moving rats. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1986;473:367-381. [PubMed ID: 3467628].
-
37.
Di Chiara G, Imperato A. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1988;85:5274-5278. [PubMed ID: 2899326].
-
38.
Pontieri FE, Tanda G, Di Chiara G. Intra-venous cocaine, morphine, and amphetamine preferentially increase extracellular dopamine in the «shell» as compared with the «core» of the rat nucleus accumbens. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1995;92:12304-12308. [PubMed ID: 8618890].
-
39.
DeMei C, Ramos M, Iitaka C, Borrelli E. Getting specialized: presynaptic and postsynaptic dopamine D2 receptors. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 2009;9:53-58. [PubMed ID: 19138563].
-
40.
Sharifzadeh M, Rezaei H, Ghamsari MR. Interactive Effects of Acute and Chronic Lithium with Dopamine Receptor Antagonists on Naloxone-Induced Jumping in Morphine-Dependent Mice. Iranian J. Pharm. Res. 2003:217-223.
-
41.
Jaber M, Robinson SW, Missale C, Caron MG. Dopamine receptors and brain function. Neuropharmacol. 1996;35:1503-1519.