Abstract
Background:
Iron deficiency is one of the most common nutrient defeciencies in the world. Iron plays an important role in the central nervous system. The aim of this study was to identify the association of iron deficiency with IQ level of students.Methods:
In this case control study, 289 randomly selected students aged eight to eleven years old were tested for iron, TIBC, Hb, and RBC indices. Iron deficient patients were referred to a psychologist to determine their IQ level with the Raven test. The IQ level of children with Iron deficiency was compared with a normal student randomly chosen and matched by age, gender, and socioeconomic status.Results:
Sixty patients had a Fe/TIBC ratio of less than 15%. The frequency of iron deficiency was 20.7%. There was no significant differences in the frequency of iron deficiency between males and females. A significant difference was not found in the IQ level between cases and controls.Conclusions:
The IQ of cases and controls did not differ significantly. It seems that there was still controversies regarding the effects of IQ and iron deficiency.Keywords
1. Background
According to the world health organization (WHO), anemia is seen in one-fourth of the world’s population with the iron deficiency being the most common cause (1). Iron deficiency is also the most common nutrient deficiency in the world and its prevalence in developing countries is up to 50% (2, 3). The role of iron in the brain could be explained by the fact that many enzymes in the central nervous system (CNS) are iron dependent, and iron could play an important role in dopaminergic pathways and in early myelination of the CNS (3-5). The learning deficits in adulthood may be due to abnormal hippocampal structure and plasticity, which could be affected by iron deficiency (2). Iron deficiency is proposed as a risk factor of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and some neurologic disorders, like febrile seizure (6-8).
Many studies suggest the relationship between iron deficiency and poor cognitive function (9-13). Moreover, in iron deficient infants, longitudinal studies showed poor cognitive outcomes, especially in early childhood (14). However, the influence of confounding factors, such as other nutritional factors and socioeconomic status, should be considered (15). On the other hand, other studies did not find any association between iron deficiency and IQ level (3, 16), and there is not enough evidence that iron therapy has a positive effect on young children’s development (14). Furthermore,the resultsof iron therapy in iron deficient children are conflicting (17). Due to the limited pool of knowledge in the region of the current study, the researchers decided to evaluate the IQ level of iron deficient students and compare them with controls matched by age, gender, and socioeconomic status.
2. Methods
This case control study was carried out in Zanjan, Northwest of Iran. Two hundred and eighty-nine students aged eight to eleven years old, from nine schools with different socioeconomic status were randomly selected. Written informed consent was taken from the parents. The selected children were physically examined and were tested for hemoglobin, red blood count (RBC) indices, serum Iron, and total iron binding capacity (TIBC). Iron deficiency was considered as serum iron to TIBC ratio of < 15%. Children, who were diagnosed with iron deficiency, were referred to a psychologist to determine their IQ level with the Raven test. Reliability and validity of the Raven’s progressive matrices were defined by previous studies in Iran (18). For each child with Iron deficiency, a normal student was randomly chosen from the same class as a control to measure the IQ. The ethics committee at Zanjan University of Medical Sciences (ZUMS) approved the study.
3. Results
Two hundred and eighty-nine students eight to eleven years old (139 males and 150 females) were enrolled in this study in four age groups. There were 21 males and 34 females aged eight years old, 38 males and 38 females aged nine years old, 41 males and 41 females aged ten years oldand 39 males and 37 females aged 11 years. Sixty patients had a Fe/TIBC ratio less than 15%. The frequency of iron deficiency was 20.7%. There was no significant differences in the frequency of iron deficiency between males and females. The frequency of iron deficiency in different age groups is shown in Table 1.
The Frequency of Iron Deficiencya
Age | 8 - 9 | 9 - 10 | 10 - 11 | 11 - 12 | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | Female | Total | Male | Female | Total | Male | Female | Total | Male | Female | Total | |
Iron deficient | 2 (9.5) | 6 (17.6) | 8 (14.5) | 12 (31.6) | 11 (28.9) | 23 (30.2) | 11 (26.8) | 10 (24.4) | 21 (25.6) | 5 (12.8) | 3 (8.1) | 8 (10.5) |
Total | 21 | 34 | 55 | 38 | 38 | 76 | 41 | 41 | 82 | 39 | 37 | 76 |
The most frequent sign and symptoms of the patients with iron deficiency was paleness (11.6%), anorexia (8.3%), pica (3.3%), and heart murmur (3.3%).
Thirty-five students with iron deficiency accepted to determine their IQ level. Thirty students without iron deficiency matched by gender and age group from the same class were selected randomly as controls.
The students’ IQ level in cases and controls are shown in Table 2.
The Students’ IQ Levela
IQ Level | Cases | Controls | Total |
---|---|---|---|
≤ 64 | - | - | - |
65 - 76 | - | - | - |
77 - 88 | 2 (5.7) | - | 2 (3.1) |
89 - 112 | 27 (77.2) | 17 (56.7) | 44 (67.7) |
113 - 124 | 5 (14.3) | 11 (36.7) | 16 (24.6) |
125 - 148 | 1 (2.8) | 2 (6.6) | 3 (4.6) |
≥ 149 | - | - | - |
There was no significant difference in the IQ level between cases and controls.
4. Discussion
Although the role of iron deficiency anemia on cognitive impairment is well documented in the literature, the influence of confounding factors should be considered. Based on this conception, many clinical trials have shown that iron supplementation could improve cognitive impairment and intellectual performance in children (9-13). In a multi-center birth cohort project in Korea, it was shown that besides iron deficiency, blood lead level was significantly associated with impaired cognitive function (19). Mubarak’s study showed a lower IQ level in iron deficient children compared to thalassemic and control groups (20).
However, some studies, especially on iron deficient children, concluded that there was no significant association between iron status and IQ level or school performance (16). Goudarzi did not find a significant difference in IQ of iron deficient and normal control groups, which is similar to the current study (3). The difference of treatment results between iron deficient anemic children and iron deficient non-anemic children was stated in Falkingham’ s study. There was no significant improvement in memory and scholastic achievement in iron deficient non-anemic children (1).
Moreover, the fact that iron supplements in young children does not show improvement in cognitive, school performance, and behavioral problems in middle childhood, it is still a matter of debate (14). It was proposed that it may be due to social background or irreversible brain damage and it is not completely clear that it can be treated with iron (14).
Also in one study, prenatal iron supplement did not have a significant effect on IQ in four-year-old children of Australia (21). Daily iron supplementation also did not increase the IQ level nor the school performance of Thai children in a double blind, randomized clinical trial (22) and other nutrient deficiencies and deprived environment was proposed as its cause. Also, IQ scores were not significantly improved in six- to eight-year-old children after taking iron supplements in Umamaheswari’s study (17).
Although iron per se could influence cognitive performance, some authors had also considered that some symptoms of iron deficient anemic children on brain could be the result of anemia rather than the iron itself (15).
4.1. Conclusions
The present study did not find a significant difference in the IQ level between iron deficient cases and controls. Although children’s IQ level was not determined after iron supplements, yet the current results are similar to some other studies. It seems that there are still controversies regarding the effects of IQ and iron deficiencies, which requires further studies on different populations and different age groups.
Acknowledgements
References
-
1.
Falkingham M, Abdelhamid A, Curtis P, Fairweather-Tait S, Dye L, Hooper L. The effects of oral iron supplementation on cognition in older children and adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr J. 2010;9:4. [PubMed ID: 20100340]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC2831810]. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2891-9-4.
-
2.
Fretham SJ, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. The role of iron in learning and memory. Adv Nutr. 2011;2(2):112-21. [PubMed ID: 22332040]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3065765]. https://doi.org/10.3945/an.110.000190.
-
3.
Goudarzi A, Mehrabi MR, Goudarzi K. The effect of iron deficiency anemia on intelligence quotient (IQ) in under 17 years old students. Pak J Biol Sci. 2008;11(10):1398-400. [PubMed ID: 18817277].
-
4.
Lozoff B. Early iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects consistent with dopaminergic dysfunction. J Nutr. 2011;141(4):740S-6S. [PubMed ID: 21346104]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3056585]. https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.110.131169.
-
5.
Walter T. Effect of iron-deficiency anemia on cognitive skills and neuromaturation in infancy and childhood. Food Nutr Bull. 2003;24(4 Suppl):S104-10. [PubMed ID: 17016952]. https://doi.org/10.1177/15648265030244S207.
-
6.
Millichap JG, Yee MM, Davidson SI. Serum ferritin in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Neurol. 2006;34(3):200-3. [PubMed ID: 16504789]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.09.001.
-
7.
Sadeghzadeh M, Khoshnevis Asl P, Mahboubi E. Iron status and febrile seizure- a case control study in children less than 3 years. Iran J Child Neurol. 2012;6(4):27-31. [PubMed ID: 24665277]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3943016].
-
8.
Sharif MR, Madani M, Tabatabaei F, Tabatabaee Z. The Relationship between Serum Vitamin D Level and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Iran J Child Neurol. 2015;9(4):48-53. [PubMed ID: 26664441]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4670977].
-
9.
Igbal K, Zafar T, Igbal Z, Usman M, Bibi H, Afreen MS, et al. Effect of Iron Deficiency Anemia on Intellectual Performance of Primary School Children in Islamabad, Pakistan. Trop J Pharm Res. 2015;14(2):287. https://doi.org/10.4314/tjpr.v14i2.14.
-
10.
Agaoglu L, Torun O, Unuvar E, Sefil Y, Demir D. Effects of iron deficiency anemia on cognitive function in children. Arzneimittelforschung. 2007;57(6A):426-30. [PubMed ID: 17691592]. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0031-1296691.
-
11.
Gupta SK, Bansal D, Malhi P, Das R. Developmental profile in children with iron deficiency anemia and its changes after therapeutic iron supplementation. Indian J Pediatr. 2010;77(4):375-9. [PubMed ID: 20428973]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12098-010-0046-9.
-
12.
More S, Shivkumar VB, Gangane N, Shende S. Effects of iron deficiency on cognitive function in school going adolescent females in rural area of central India. Anemia. 2013;2013:819136. [PubMed ID: 24386560]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3872396]. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/819136.
-
13.
Metallinos-Katsaras E, Valassi-Adam E, Dewey KG, Lonnerdal B, Stamoulakatou A, Pollitt E. Effect of iron supplementation on cognition in Greek preschoolers. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2004;58(11):1532-42. [PubMed ID: 15226754]. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602005.
-
14.
Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C. A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr. 2001;131(2S-2):649S-66S. discussion 666S-668S. [PubMed ID: 11160596]. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/131.2.649S.
-
15.
Grein J. The cognitive effects of iron deficiency in non-anemic children. Nutr Noteworth. 2001;4(1).
-
16.
Dissanayake DS, Kumarasiri PV, Nugegoda DB, Dissanayake DM. The association of iron status with educational performance and intelligence among adolescents. Ceylon Med J. 2009;54(3):75-9. [PubMed ID: 19999786].
-
17.
Umamaheswari K, Bhaskaran M, Krishnamurthy G, Vasudevan H, Vasudevan K. Effect of iron and zinc deficiency on short term memory in children. Indian Pediatr. 2011;48(4):289-93. [PubMed ID: 20972302].
-
18.
Baraheni MN. Raven's Progressive Matrices as applied to Iranian children. Educ Psychol Measure. 1974;34(4):983-8.
-
19.
Jeong KS, Park H, Ha E, Hong YC, Ha M, Park H, et al. Evidence that cognitive deficit in children is associated not only with iron deficiency, but also with blood lead concentration: a preliminary study. J Trace Elem Med Biol. 2015;29:336-41. [PubMed ID: 25224739]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtemb.2014.08.014.
-
20.
Mubarak A, Fadel W, Said S, Hammar MA. Profile of Behavior and IQ in Anemic Children. CNS Spectr. 2010;15(12):631-8. [PubMed ID: 24726084]. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852912000089.
-
21.
Zhou SJ, Gibson RA, Crowther CA, Baghurst P, Makrides M. Effect of iron supplementation during pregnancy on the intelligence quotient and behavior of children at 4 y of age: long-term follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83(5):1112-7. [PubMed ID: 16685054]. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/83.5.1112.
-
22.
Sungthong R, Mo-suwan L, Chongsuvivatwong V, Geater AF. Once-weekly and 5-days a week iron supplementation differentially affect cognitive function but not school performance in Thai children. J Nutr. 2004;134(9):2349-54. [PubMed ID: 15333727]. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/134.9.2349.