A Comparison of Early Versus Late Nap Times During the Night Shift Regarding the Psychomotor Performance Required for Safe Driving: A Pilot Study

authors:

avatar Soheil Saadat 1 , avatar Iraj Alimohammadi 2 , avatar Mojgan Karbakhsh 3 , avatar Hassan Ashayeri 4 , avatar Mahsa Fayaz 5 , avatar Farideh Sadeghian 6 , *

Sina Trauma and Surgery Research Center, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
Occupational Health Research Center, School of Public Health, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
Department of Community Medicine, School of Medicine, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
Department of Basic Sciences in Rehabilitation, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, Shahroud University of Medical Sciences, Shahroud, Iran
Center for Health Related Social and Behavioral Sciences Research, Shahroud University of Medical Sciences, Shahroud, Iran

how to cite: Saadat S, Alimohammadi I , Karbakhsh M , Ashayeri H , Fayaz M, et al. A Comparison of Early Versus Late Nap Times During the Night Shift Regarding the Psychomotor Performance Required for Safe Driving: A Pilot Study. Shiraz E-Med J. 2019;20(11):e86980. https://doi.org/10.5812/semj.86980.

Abstract

Background:

Sleep deprivation leads to fatigue and sleepiness among health care workers and can result in road traffic collisions (RTC) after their shift. Planned napping is one of the main solutions proposed to avoid this problem.

Objectives:

The present study aimed at assessing the effect of two nap times during the night shift on psychomotor driving skills among night shift nurses.

Methods:

A cohort study was conducted on 19 night shift nurses. The nurses completed the eight tests of the validated traffic psychological assessment battery of Vienna test system (VTS) before the night shift at 6 PM, and they repeated the same set of tests after the night shift at 7 AM. Data were compared based on nap times (00:00 - 03:00 AM versus 03:00 - 06:00 AM), which were chosen by the night shift nurses.

Results:

Nurses aged 23 to 36 years old participated in this study. Their driving experience was 6.1 ± 3.4 years, and 34% reported RTCs in the previous year. The nurses who chose to take naps from 00:00 to 03:00 showed a significantly improved time-movement anticipation ability in driving compared to those who napped from 03:00 AM to 06:00 AM (P value = 0.005).

Conclusions:

With respect to the time-movement anticipation test, napping during 00:00 to 03:00 AM provided better results compared to napping at 03:00 to 06:00 AM. Anticipation is a highly important psychomotor ability for safe driving.

1. Background

Healthy adults need 7.5 to 8.8 hours of sleep per night (1). Sleep deprivation is the most common health problem among night shift nurses (2). Gold et al. reported that 30.9% of the nurses had experienced a near-miss (an unplanned event) while driving home after a night shift during the preceding year (3). Impaired alertness and decreased attention (4), sleepiness, and fatigue (5) are the major adverse effects of sleep deprivation, which can lead to road traffic collisions (RTCs) (6).

A study indicated most of the RTCs among night shift health cares occurred between 8 AM and 9 AM in the morning after a night shift when they were driving home (7).

Based on findings of the previous studies, night time napping is an effective method to increase alertness, improve cognitive performance (8), and reduce sleepiness (9). Planned napping at the workplace is one way to reduce fatigue and sleepiness among health care workers (10). Night shift nurses frequently experience sleepiness, however, planned napping can decrease both sleepiness (11, 12) and the risk of RTC (13) when driving home after shifts. Nurses taking a nap during their night shifts is reported to diminish sleepiness while driving home after their shifts (13). Silva-Costa et al. reported that the efficacy of napping during night shifts is similar to that of night time sleep at home (14).

A study by Gillberg showed that an hour nap at 4:30 AM can improve performance at the end of the shift (7 AM) compared to no nap (15). Taking a nap at 2 AM and 3 AM is reported to improve the reaction time and reduce fatigue after the shift (11, 16, 17).

A main challenge of shift work is the conflict between displaced work hours and an internal body clock in the brain that produces circadian rhythms; it exists in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of the hypothalamus (18). This body clock produces a signal that results in a pronounced 24-hour alternation in all physiological and psychological functions such as body temperature, alertness, sleepiness, and hormone levels. Melatonin has a high point at ~04:00 AM and a low point at ~16:00 PM, and is closely associated with temperature and alertness (19). The circadian adjustment of shift workers is slow, about one hour every day. If no adjustment occurs, work at the circadian nadir will be performed at low levels of physiological activation, alertness, and behavioral efficiency (20).

It has been shown that more than 90% of RTCs are due to human errors and driver behavior (21, 22). Thus, traffic psychology tests have become highly important to assess safe driving (21).

2. Objectives

In this study, by using a validated computerized test system (23), we examined the relationship between two nap times (00:00 - 03:00 AM versus 03:00 - 06:00 AM) and the psychomotor skills required for safe driving after a night shift for the first time in Iran. These skills include attention, concentration, reactive stress tolerance, perceptual speed, peripheral perception, visual pursuit ability, visuo-motor coordination, and time-movement anticipation.

3. Methods

3.1. Participants and Procedure

A cohort study was conducted on 19 night shift female nurses working at the Sina Hospital in Tehran (Iran) during 2015 and 2016 through an announcement in all departments. In this hospital, night shift nurses work 12 hours from 7 PM to 7 AM, and they can take three hour naps from 00:00 to 03:00 AM or from 03:00 to 06:00 AM by their choice.

Participants in our study also chose their nap times based on their preferences. Eight participants chose to nap from 00:00 to 03:00 AM (group one) and 11 chose to nap from 03:00 to 06:00 AM (group two).

Criteria for inclusion were as follow: healthy female nurses aged 20 to 40; at least one year of work experience; holding a driver’s license; being a frequent driver, and working only one shift during the 24 hours preceding the study.

Nurses attended a training session to practice with the traffic psychological assessment battery of Vienna test system (VTS). Then, on the day of the study, they completed the VTS test set one hour before the beginning of the night shift at 6 PM. Then, they completed the same tests again at the end of the night shift in the following morning at 7 AM before returning home. The participants were asked to abstain from drinking coffee or taking any medication that could affect their sleep during the test sessions.

3.2. Data Collection Tools

The demographic and circadian type inventory (CTI) questionnaires were used for data collection. CTI questionnaire measures rhythm, stability, and amplitude of circadian rhythm. Flexible/rigid (FR) states the flexibility of sleeping habits. Rigid types denote to those who cannot sleep at irregular hours. Languid/vigorous (LV) explains the amplitude of circadian cycles. Languid types cannot overcome sleepiness in the condition of lack of sleep, while vigorous types are more alert during the day and need less sleep than languid types. Vigorous and flexible types show better adaptation with shift work (24).

Also, traffic psychological battery of the Vienna test system was used for assess psychomotor ability of driving after night shift. The eight VTS tests used in this study were as follow: the determination test (DT); cognitrone test (COG); reaction time test (RT); adaptive tachistoscopic traffic perception test (ATAVT); peripheral perception test (PP); visual pursuit test (LVT); two hand coordination test (2HAND); and time-movement anticipation test (ZBA). The validity and reliability of the tests have been reported in several studies (23, 25). Detailed explanation of the tests is provided in the Supplementary File Appendix 1.

3.3. Statistical Inference

To better understand the results, we defined scores as follows:

Scores obtained before the shift (SOBS); scores obtained after the shift (SOAS); changes in SOAS, compared to SOBS (CABS), and difference of CABS between the two study groups (DCABS). An interpretation was made based on the comparison of DCABS between the two groups. Therefore, the DCABS was calculated for every test and compared between the two study groups, using independent student’s t-test and Wilcoxon Rank test.

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 20.0. Significance level was set at 0.05.

3.4. Ethical Consideration

The study protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Tehran University of Medical Sciences and in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the 1975 Declaration of Helsinki (reference number: IR TUMS.REC.2015.1984- 7/12/2015). All the participants provided written informed consent.

4. Results

Table 1 demonstrates the comparison of baseline characteristics between the study groups. There was no statistically significant difference in the baseline characteristics of the study groups.

Table 1.

Baseline Characteristics of the Study Groupsa

CharacteristicsGroup 1, 00 - 03 AM Nap, (N = 8)Group 2, 03 - 06 AM Nap, (N = 11)P Value
Age29.5 ± 4.1428.73 ± 4.100.69b
Driving experience7.00 ± 2.515.27 ± 4.340.33b
Sleep duration within last 24 hours8.87 ± 1.968.73 ± 2.410.89b
Marital status0.66c
Single5 (62.5)7 (63.3)
Married3 (37.5)4 (36.4)
Circadian rhythm
Flexible2 (25)2 (18.2)0.57c
Rigid6 (75)9 (81.8)
Langui1 (12.5)1 (9.1)0.68c
Vigorous7 (87.5)10 (90.9)
RTC history0.17c
No4 (50)9 (81.8)
Yes4 (50)2 (18.2)

Table 2 presents the mean CABS in the study groups as well as the DCABS.

Table 2.

The Mean Score Change (CABS) of Psychomotor Abilities of Driving Variables as well as the Mean Differences (DCABS) Among the Study Groups

TestsVariablesUnitCABS, Type of NapDCABS, Mean (SE)P Value
Group 1, 0:00 - 03:00 AMGroup 2, 03:00 - 06:00 AM
ZBA (time-movement anticipation)Median deviation time SQRT transformeds-0.26 ± 0.210.05 ± 0.21-0.31 (0.097)0.005a
2 Hand (viso motor coordination)Overall mean duration (2Hand1)s0.46 ± 6.30-4.41 ± 5.334.89 (2.6)0.087a
Overall percent error duration (2Hand2)--0.11 ± 1.86-0.24 ± 1.190.11 (0.69)0.88a
ATAVT (perceptual speed)Overview-2.25 ± 3.92-0.64 ± 2.542.89 (1.48)0.068a
DT (choice reaction time)Correct response (DT1)n2.37 ± 12.962.9 ± 12.536.25 (-15.81)0.68a
Incorrect response (DT2)n0.25 ± 3.10-0.40 ± 4.880.43 (1.91)0.82a
Omitted response (DT3)n-0.12 ± 2.95-1.70 ± 3.972.69 (2.08)0.21a
Median reaction time (DT4)s-0.01 ± 0.02-0.002 ± 0.04-0.01 (0.01)0.47a
LVT (visual orientation)Score [based on viewing times] (LVT1)-0.25 ± 1.670.30 ± 3.230.16 (1.12)0.89a
Score [based on working times] (LVT2)-0.87 ± 1.730.60 ± 2.800.58 (2.34)0.44b
PP (peripheral perception)Field of vision (PP1)°-4.26 ± 20.301.2 ± 4.730.16 (13.37)0.8b
Tracking deviation (PP2)-0.8 ± 0.96-0.11 ± 0.840.23 (0.94)0.49b
RT (simple reaction time)Mean reaction time (RT1)ms-14.37 ± 63.658.7 ± 48.71-20.28 (25.34)0.43a
Mean motor time (RT2)ms-1.5 ± 28.9731.10 ± 80.4915.58 (62.04)0.49b
Cog (selective attention)Sum hits (Cog1)n-0.13 ± 1.25-0.30 ± 0.95-0.26 (1.05)0.72b
Correct rejections (Cog2)n-0.13 ± 1.360.40 ± 1.710.16 (1.50)0.60b
Mean time correct rejection (Cog3)s0.09 ± 0.400.05 ± 0.280.06 (0.15)0.72a

The mean CABS in case of the “median deviation time” (ZBA test) was negative in group one (-0.26 ± 0.21) and positive in group two (0.05 ± 0.21). The DCABS was statistically significant in case of this variable (P = 0.005).

The DCABS in the subtests of other VTS tests (COG, LVT, RT, DT, ATAVT, PP, and 2Hand) failed to reach significant level (P > 0.05) (Table 2).

5. Discussion

The present study aimed at determining the change in the psychomotor skills of driving in nurses after a night shift, according to their nap time during their shift.

Each participant passed the tests before and after the night shift. The CABS is influenced by fatigue and sleepiness, as well as the learning curve mechanism. Therefore, if one group shows less improvement (or deterioration) in the test scores obtained after the shift compared to the other group, then, we can conclude that the timing of the nap taken by that group was not as effective as the other group.

Based on the results, improvement in the participants’ ability of time-movement anticipation was significantly higher in group one, compared to group two in the ZBA test. The ZBA test could be related to the ability of estimating the time to collision of two vehicles that are moving at a constant speed (26). In this test, a ball moving slowly appears on the monitor. Then, in an unexpected moment, this ball disappears and two red lines appear; one line is positioned at the point where the ball has disappeared, and the other is considered the target line. The participant should press one button the second he/she thinks the ball crosses the target line (Figure 1).

The advantage of a 00:00 to 03:00 nap time compared to 00:03 to 00:06 may be explained by sleep inertia, a physiological state characterized by a deficit of motor skills directly after rapidly waking up and damaged alertness may affect physical and mental performance (27, 28). Another explanation could be the quantity and quality of sleep during nap opportunity. A study of nurses revealed that those who napped from 00:00 to 03:00 AM slept 147.5 minutes, while those who napped from 03:00 to 06:00 AM slept 112.0 minutes only. Shorter napping during this period could be related to medical procedures in the hospital before 7 AM that increase noise levels and interrupt the naps. Furthermore, anxiety about getting ready to leave (29) for another job may be another reason for shorter naps during the 03:00 to 06:00 AM naps.

In this study, we did not detect a statistically significant difference between the two nap groups in the change of test scores before and after the shift with respect to other psychomotor driving abilities. These tests include two hand coordination, peripheral perception, selective attention, visual pursuit, reactive stress tolerance, reaction time, and perceptual speed. This could be due to the limited sample size of our study or the lack of advantage between the two nap times. Studies with larger sample sizes are required to identify the equality of nap times with regards to the above-mentioned tests.

Previous researches directed to measure fitness to drive had focused on the RTC rates. However, this measure has several disadvantages. RTCs rarely occur, follow poison distribution, and are multi-causal (23). Other methods for measuring the risk of RTC including information gathered through self-reporting, insurance companies, and official sources are significantly different in agreement levels (30). Comparison of self-reported and recorded driving memories information revealed that 80% of minor RTCs and most of the near-accidents would be forgotten in two weeks (31). Several studies have designed standardized driving tests as an alternative measure of respondents’ fitness to drive (32, 33). VTS is among the best-known tests and its validity and reliability for measuring fitness to drive have been reported in several studies (25, 34). The VTS battery used in our study is a standard computer system that assesses essential psychomotor requirements for safe driving. It is legally used in 25 countries to assess drivers who commit violations (35).

To our knowledge, this is the first study that has utilized a reliable objective tool to compare the association of nap time with fitness to drive after night shift.

The limitation of our study was its small sample size. Moreover, we did not consider job difficulty and the personality traits of the participants. In addition, the quality of the nap and the environmental situation of the nurses’ rest room were not documented among the individuals.

5.1. Conclusions

Our study revealed that time-movement anticipation ability of night shift nurses who took a nap at 03:00 to 06:00 AM was deteriorated compared to a similar group of nurses who took a nap at 00:00 to 03:00 AM; considering the fact that time-movement anticipation is an important factor for safe driving, hazard control, and RTC prevention. We can conclude that night shift nurses who nap at 03:00 to 06:00 AM are at a higher risk of RTC while driving home after a night shift. However, further studies with larger sample sizes are needed to investigate the effect of nap types on the other psychomotor skills needed for safe driving.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Banks S, Dinges DF. Chronic sleep deprivation. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. 5th ed. Louis, Missouri: Elsevier Health Sciences; 2011. p. 67-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-1-4160-6645-3.00006-2.

  • 2.

    Johnson AL, Jung L, Song Y, Brown KC, Weaver MT, Richards KC. Sleep deprivation and error in nurses who work the night shift. J Nurs Adm. 2014;44(1):17-22. [PubMed ID: 24316614]. https://doi.org/10.1097/NNA.0000000000000016.

  • 3.

    Gold DR, Rogacz S, Bock N, Tosteson TD, Baum TM, Speizer FE, et al. Rotating shift work, sleep, and accidents related to sleepiness in hospital nurses. Am J Public Health. 1992;82(7):1011-4. [PubMed ID: 1609900]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC1694075]. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.82.7.1011.

  • 4.

    Oken BS, Salinsky MC, Elsas SM. Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: Physiological basis and measurement. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117(9):1885-901. [PubMed ID: 16581292]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC2865224]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.01.017.

  • 5.

    Ruggiero JS, Redeker NS. Effects of napping on sleepiness and sleep-related performance deficits in night-shift workers: A systematic review. Biol Res Nurs. 2014;16(2):134-42. [PubMed ID: 23411360]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4079545]. https://doi.org/10.1177/1099800413476571.

  • 6.

    Steele MT, Ma OJ, Watson WA, Thomas HJ, Muelleman RL. The occupational risk of motor vehicle collisions for emergency medicine residents. Acad Emerg Med. 1999;6(10):1050-3. [PubMed ID: 10530665]. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.1999.tb01191.x.

  • 7.

    Scott LD, Hwang WT, Rogers AE, Nysse T, Dean GE, Dinges DF. The relationship between nurse work schedules, sleep duration, and drowsy driving. Sleep. 2007;30(12):1801-7. [PubMed ID: 18246989]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC2276124]. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/30.12.1801.

  • 8.

    Takeyama H, Kubo T, Itani T. The nighttime nap strategies for improving night shift work in workplace. Ind Health. 2005;43(1):24-9. [PubMed ID: 15732300]. https://doi.org/10.2486/indhealth.43.24.

  • 9.

    Lovato N, Lack L. The effects of napping on cognitive functioning. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:155-66. [PubMed ID: 21075238]. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00009-9.

  • 10.

    [No authors listed]. Health care worker fatigue and patient safety. Sentinel Event Alert. 2011;(48):1-4. [PubMed ID: 22171415].

  • 11.

    Smith-Coggins R, Howard SK, Mac DT, Wang C, Kwan S, Rosekind MR, et al. Improving alertness and performance in emergency department physicians and nurses: the use of planned naps. Ann Emerg Med. 2006;48(5):596-604. 604 e1-3. [PubMed ID: 17052562]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annemergmed.2006.02.005.

  • 12.

    Geiger-Brown J, Sagherian K, Zhu S, Wieroniey MA, Blair L, Warren J, et al. CE: original research: Napping on the night shift: a two-hospital implementation project. Am J Nurs. 2016;116(5):26-33. [PubMed ID: 27082421]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4889223]. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NAJ.0000482953.88608.80.

  • 13.

    Garbarino S, Mascialino B, Penco MA, Squarcia S, De Carli F, Nobili L, et al. Professional shift-work drivers who adopt prophylactic naps can reduce the risk of car accidents during night work. Sleep. 2004;27(7):1295-302. [PubMed ID: 15586782]. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/27.7.1295.

  • 14.

    Silva-Costa A, Rotenberg L, Griep RH, Fischer FM. Napping on the night shift among nursing staff: potential benefits for workers' health. Escola Anna Nery. 2015;19(1). https://doi.org/10.5935/1414-8145.20150005.

  • 15.

    Gillberg M. The effects of two alternative timings of a one-hour nap on early morning performance. Biol Psychol. 1984;19(1):45-54. [PubMed ID: 6478003]. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(84)90009-7.

  • 16.

    Purnell MT, Feyer AM, Herbison GP. The impact of a nap opportunity during the night shift on the performance and alertness of 12-h shift workers. J Sleep Res. 2002;11(3):219-27. [PubMed ID: 12220318]. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00309.x.

  • 17.

    Lovato N, Lack L, Ferguson S, Tremaine R. The effects of a 30-min nap during night shift following a prophylactic sleep in the afternoon. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2009;7(1):34-42. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1479-8425.2009.00382.x.

  • 18.

    Klein D, Moore R, Reppert S. Suprachiasmatic nucleus: the mind's clock. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991.

  • 19.

    Czeisler CA, Weitzman E, Moore-Ede MC, Zimmerman JC, Knauer RS. Human sleep: Its duration and organization depend on its circadian phase. Science. 1980;210(4475):1264-7. [PubMed ID: 7434029]. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7434029.

  • 20.

    Wever RA. Phase shifts of human circadian rhythms due to shifts of artificial Zeitgebers. Chronobiologia. 1980;7(3):303-27. [PubMed ID: 6108838].

  • 21.

    Beierle B. Psychologische und technische analyse von LKW-Verkehrsunfällen. 1995.

  • 22.

    Shappell SA, Wiegmann DA. Human factors investigation and analysis of accidents and incidents. Encyclopedia of forensic sciences. 2013. p. 440-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-382165-2.00146-x.

  • 23.

    Risser R, Chaloupka C, Grundler W, Sommer M, Hausler J, Kaufmann C. Using non-linear methods to investigate the criterion validity of traffic-psychological test batteries. Accid Anal Prev. 2008;40(1):149-57. [PubMed ID: 18215543]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2006.12.018.

  • 24.

    Di Milia L, Smith PA, Folkard S. A validation of the revised circadian type inventory in a working sample. Pers Individ Differ. 2005;39(7):1293-305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.04.012.

  • 25.

    Sommer M, Herle M, Häusler J, Risser R, Schützhofer B, Chaloupka C. Cognitive and personality determinants of fitness to drive. Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2008;11(5):362-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2008.03.001.

  • 26.

    Hayward JC. Near miss determination through use of a scale of danger. 1972.

  • 27.

    Tassi P, Muzet A. Sleep inertia. Sleep Med Rev. 2000;4(4):341-53. [PubMed ID: 12531174]. https://doi.org/10.1053/smrv.2000.0098.

  • 28.

    Wertz AT, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP Jr. Effects of sleep inertia on cognition. JAMA. 2006;295(2):163-4. [PubMed ID: 16403927]. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.295.2.163.

  • 29.

    Snyder CW. National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA) NASA. Encyclopedia of Planetary Science: Springer; 1997.

  • 30.

    Szlyk JP, Seiple W, Viana M. Relative effects of age and compromised vision on driving performance. Hum Factors. 1995;37(2):430-6. [PubMed ID: 7642186]. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872095779064645.

  • 31.

    Chapman P, Underwood G. Forgetting near-accidents: the roles of severity, culpability and experience in the poor recall of dangerous driving situations. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2000;14(1):31-44. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(200001)14:1<31::aid-acp622>3.0.co;2-9.

  • 32.

    Lundqvist A, Gerdle B, Ronnberg J. Neuropsychological aspects of driving after a stroke?in the simulator and on the road. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2000;14(2):135-50. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(200003/04)14:2<135::aid-acp628>3.0.co;2-s.

  • 33.

    Sommer M, Arendasy M, Olbrich A, Schuhfried G. [Qualitaetsverbesserung in der verkehrspsychologischen Diagnostik mit neuronalen Netzen: Eine Pilotstudie/Quality improvement in traffic psychological assessment through artificial neural networks: A pilot study]. Zeitschrift für Verkehrssicherheit. 2004;50(4). German.

  • 34.

    Risser R. Validity of driver's diagnostics and selection. Assessing the driver. 1997. p. 89-101.

  • 35.

    Amado S, Ulupınar P. The effects of conversation on attention peripheral detection: Is talking with a passenger and talking on the cell phone different? Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2005;8(6):383-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2005.05.001.