Investigating the Characteristics of Intelligence Quotient in Individuals with Intellectual Disability in Rafsanjan, 2019

authors:

avatar Parvin Agha Mohammad Hasani ORCID 1 , avatar Mohammadreza Mokhtaree ORCID 2 , 3 , avatar Mohsen Rezaeian ORCID 4 , avatar Arman Jalaladdini 5 , *

Department of Psychiatry, Rafsanjan University of Medical Sciences, Rafsanjan, Iran
Social Determinants of Health Research Center, Rafsanjan University of Medical Sciences, Rafsanjan, Iran
Education and Psychology Department, Kerman Branch, Islamic Azad University, Kerman, Iran
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Medicine, Rafsanjan University of Medical Sciences, Rafsanjan, Iran
Rafsanjan University of Medical Sciences, Rafsanjan, Iran

how to cite: Agha Mohammad Hasani P, Mokhtaree M , Rezaeian M, Jalaladdini A. Investigating the Characteristics of Intelligence Quotient in Individuals with Intellectual Disability in Rafsanjan, 2019. J Adv Immunopharmacol. 2021;1(1):e113794. https://doi.org/10.5812/tms.113794.

Abstract

Background:

Comprehensive and reliable information about intellectual disability (ID) and its association with environmental and demographic variables is scarce.

Objectives:

This study aimed to investigate the characteristics of intelligence quotient (IQ) in children with ID.

Methods:

In this descriptive cross-sectional study, all children with ID under the care of the welfare organization and special needs schools of the city of Rafsanjan were included in the study by census sampling (n = 207). After obtaining informed consent, parents completed a checklist including participants’ age, gender, birth month, birth season, and birth order, and also parents’ age and education level. Data were analyzed by one-way ANOVA and chi-square tests.

Results:

Out of 207 children, 107 (51.7%) were male, 100 (48.3%) were female, 36.2% were first-born, 14.5% were born in May, and 5.3% were born in March (the most-the least). The relationship of IQ to birth season and month was not significant (P > 0.05). Among the children, 39.6% were offspring of consanguineous marriages, but no significant relationship was observed between their IQ and consanguineous marriages (P > 0.05). The level of ID was correlated with parents’ age (P < 0.05). A significant relationship was also found between IQ and the number of offspring (P = 0.004). All cases of severe ID were seen in girls and a significant relationship was detected between IQ and gender (P = 0.0001).

Conclusions:

Individuals with ID have an IQ below 70. ID in children was correlated with the parents’ old age and low education level, but not with their birth season. However, given the higher prevalence of infectious diseases especially viral infections in cold months, it is only sensible to educate couples about how to protect the health of mother and fetus during these periods.

1. Background

In general, individuals with intellectual disability (ID) have an IQ below 70; however, clinical discretion may be necessary for those who have a somewhat higher IQ but severe impairment in adaptive functioning (1). The American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD) defines ID as significant limitations in intellectual functioning and adaptive behaviors that begin before the age of 18 and negatively affect social and conceptual skills. In this definition, adaptive behaviors refer to the activities that are typically expected from a person of a particular age (2).

Causes of ID are divided into two categories, genetic and non-genetic, most of which fall into the genetic category. The genetic causes of ID include abnormalities in the number and structure of chromosomes and genomic and single-gene disorders. This is a worldwide issue and has nothing to do with any specific country or particular region (3, 4). Chromosomal abnormalities have been reported to account for about 6.4% of ID cases (5, 6).

Since the prevalence of genetic disorders and autosomal recessive diseases increases with the rate of consanguineous marriages, they are expected to play a critical role in the incidence of ID in countries like Iran, where the rate of consanguineous marriages is reported to be as high as 38% (7). In Iran, a study conducted in Ardabil estimated the prevalence of ID at 3.6%, which is higher than the global average (8).

Many factors can contribute to the incidence of ID. For many years, experts have been intrigued by the potential role of the birth season in the child’s physical and psychological characteristics and their susceptibility to ID. Such a relationship can be related to the role of the season and environmental factors in the early stages of life. For example, it has been suggested that since the first and third trimesters of pregnancy play an important role in the formation and growth of fetal organs, spending these trimesters in the cold season may affect fetal growth, because the mother would be more exposed to communicable and infectious diseases and depression and also less exposed to sunlight. The relation between decreased vitamin D absorption and disorders such as autism is particularly well discussed (9), as vitamin D has been shown to play a key role in fetal brain development through involvement in neuronal differentiation, axon binding, dopamine reception, immune function, and transcriptional control of many genes (10). Some neonatal specialists believe that neonatal jaundice is associated with exposure to sunlight, which is a function of the pregnancy season, although evidence on this association is scarce. In this regard, Sajjadian et al. (11) has reported that the birth season acts as an independent factor in the incidence of neonatal jaundice and that hyperbilirubinemia occurs more frequently in winter than in other seasons. The study by Mohammadi et al. (12) suggested the effects of the season as an environmental factor on diabetes incidence.

Also, a study in the United States has reported that mothers over 34 years old have a higher rate of giving birth to children with autism and without learning disabilities, and fathers older than 34 years of age are associated with an increased incidence of learning disabilities without autism in their children (13).

Birth order also has a profound association with the incidence of diseases. According to Adler (14), one of the pioneers of research in this area, birth order is significantly associated with children’s development and personality. More recent studies have focused on the relationship of birth order to intelligence, career choices, and success in adulthood (15). Also, it has been reported that first-borns often choose occupations that require high precision, such as academic, medical, law, engineering, computer, or accounting careers (15, 16).

2. Objectives

This study aimed to investigate the characteristics of IQ in children with ID in the city of Rafsanjan to provide evidence to confirm or refute the aforementioned hypothesis.

3. Methods

The population of this descriptive cross-sectional study included all children with ID who were studying in the special needs schools of Rafsanjan or were under the care of the governmental and private centers supervised by the welfare organization in Rafsanjan (n = 207).

All subjects were included in the study by census sampling. After gaining permission from the research ethics committee (code: IR.RUMS.REC.1395.155), the purpose of the study was explained to the parents of the children participants and informed consent was obtained. Finally, 207 participants entered the study. A checklist was prepared to collect the desired information including age, gender, birth season, and parents’ education level. The parents completed the checklist regarding the child’s age, gender, birth order, IQ score, and birth month, and regarding parents’ demographic characteristics such as age, education level, and consanguineous marriage. The collected data were analyzed in SPSS 18 using one-way ANOVA and chi-square tests.

4. Results

A total of 207 participants (mean age = 13.44 ± 4.076 years; age range = 6-25 years) participated in the study. Of these, 107 (51.7%) were male and 100 (48.3%) were female. For the children, the months with the highest and lowest frequencies of birth were May with 30 births (14.5%) and March with 11 births (5.3%), respectively. Of the 207 mothers surveyed, 87 (42%) had an elementary education and 86 (41.5%) had a high school diploma. The frequency of ID among children did not show any significant relationship to birth month, birth order, and mother’s or father’s education level (P > 0.05). The parents of participants with severe ID had a significantly higher mean age than other parents (P = 0.007 and P = 0.002). The families of participants with mild ID had a significantly higher mean number of members than other families (P = 0.004) (Table 1). The girls had a higher rate of mild and severe ID (P = 0.0001) (Table 2). No significant difference was observed between the frequency of consanguineous and non-consanguineous couples with intellectually disabled children (P = 0.229) (Table 2). This test also found no significant relationship between ID and birth season (Table 3).

Table 1.

Parents’ Mean age and Number of Offspring

IDMother’s Age, Mean ± SDFather’s Age, Mean ± SDNumber of Offspring, Mean ± SD
Severe 46.82 ± 6.8150.18 ± 8.683.36 ± 1.53
Moderate 41.59 ± 7.3545.09 ± 7.222.77 ± 1.40
Mild 37.60 ± 6.9340.0 ± 7.234.07 ± 2.46
Total 41.57 ± 7.4445.0 ± 7.492.89 ± 1.53
ANOVA F2,204 = 5.04; P = 0.007F2,204 = 6.30; P = 0.002F2,204 = 5.78; P = 0.004
Table 2.

Frequency of Individuals with ID based on Gender and Parents’ Marriage

IDBoyGirlConsanguineous MarriageNon-Consanguineous MarriageTotal
Severe011 (11.0)2 (2.4)9 (7.2)11 (5.3)
Moderate 106 (99.1)75 (75.0)73 (89.0)108 (86.4)181 (87.4)
Mild 1 (0.9)14 (14.0)7 (8.5)8 (6.4)15 (7.2)
Total 107 (100)100 (100.0)82 (100.0)125 (100.0)207 (100.0)
Chi-square testχ = 27.37; df = 2; P = 0.0001χ = 2.436; df = 2; P = 0.292
Table 3.

Frequency of Individuals with ID Based on Birth Season

IDSpringSummerAutumnWinterTotal
Severe 3 (5.2)3 (5.9)2 (3.6)3 (7.0)11 (5.3)
Moderate 50 (86.2)46 (90.2)46 (83.6)39 (90.7)181 (87.4)
Mild 5 (8.6)2 (3.9)7 (12.7)3 (2.3)15 (7.2)
Total 58 (100.0)51 (100.0)55 (100.0)58 (100.0)207 (100.0)
Chi-square testChi-square = not valid

5. Discussion

This study examined the demographic characteristics of 207 children with mild to severe ID and their parents. One of the methods to determine the level of learning of individuals, especially children, is intelligence quotient, which is measured by intelligence tests, including the Raven test. According to the American Psychiatric Association (APA), IQs below 70 are considered retarded (1).

In this study, 51.7% of children with ID were male and 48.3% were female. This slightly higher rate of incidence of ID in males than in females is consistent with the findings of the studies of Akhavan et al. (17) and Ayoglu et al. (18), showing this ratio as 55.6 to 44.4, 54.11 to 45.88, and 62.5 to 37.5, respectively. However, it is inconsistent with Noori’s (19) study in Rafsanjan, reporting a 45.7 to 54.3 male to female ratio, and Koirala et al.’s (20) study, indicating this ratio as 42 to 58. Most of these discrepancies can be attributed to differences in the studied populations. In the case of Noori et al.’s study (19), which has been conducted on the same population, the difference seems to be related to the date of research (2014 versus 2019).

Also, note that although the present study observed a slightly higher number of males with ID than their female counterparts, all of the cases with severe ID were female. Similar to the general population, there seem to be gender differences concerning several types of psychiatric diagnoses among older individuals with ID (21). More research is needed to establish if the occurrence of the disease is due to gender differences, inclination to seek care, healthcare utilization patterns, or ability to correctly identify disorders.

In this study, a plurality of individuals with ID (36.2%) was first-born. However, no particular birth order was significantly related to ID. Several studies have also reported an association between birth order and mental disorders (22, 23).

In a study by Turner et al. (24), a V-shaped relationship was observed between birth order and autism (at the levels falling in the domain of ID). In a study by Lord, the nonverbal intelligence score was found to decrease with the increase of birth order (25).

A study by Martin and Horriat (26) on autistic children also found that the IQof these children decreased with the increase of birth order. Scholars, such as Adler, believe that birth order can play an important role in the emergence of diseases. He specifically showed that birth order was significantly associated with child development and personality (14).

The present study found no significant relationship between children’s intelligence and parents’ education level. In this study, 83.6% of the mothers and 79.7% of the fathers had an elementary or high school education, consistent with the findings of Noori et al. (19). On the contrary, He et al.’s study (27) found a significant relationship between children’s low intelligence and parents’ low education. Such a relationship may be related to the poor awareness of low-educated parents regarding how the intelligence of their children can be affected by genetics (28), pregnancy (29), childbirth, and neonatal problems (30).

The results of this study showed a significant inverse relationship between the intelligence of children and the age of their parents. In other words, children with lower intelligence were more likely to have older parents. This is consistent with the finding of many studies (19, 31-36).

Note that in Zhang’s study (34), it was the father’s age that was associated with low intelligence, not the mother’s. The research results suggested that changes in women’s chromosomes after the age of 30 might result in giving birth to children with ID (37).

Sandin et al. (38) believes that the very young or very old age of parents is highly involved in the incidence of ID in children. It is widely accepted that parental age is associated with the birth of infants with various disabilities, including low intelligence. However, this study did not investigate the pregnancy age and cannot support or refute the existence of such an association.

In this study, 39.6% of the children participants were the offspring of consanguineous marriages. However, the relationship between different levels of ID and consanguineous marriages was not significant. The fact that consanguineous marriages can lead to the birth of children with intelligence problems has been shown in many studies (8, 19, 31, 32), highlighting the importance of genetic counseling before marriage.

This study also investigated possible associations of ID with birth month and season. Regarding the population of this study, the months with the highest and lowest frequencies of birth were May with 30 births (14.5%) and March with 11 births (5.3%), respectively, and no significant difference was found between different seasons and months in terms of children’s birth rate. Likewise, a study by Kastley (39) found no relationship between birth season or month and intelligence. However, Zerbo et al. (40) reported that children born in the winter were more likely to have autism. Liu et al. (41) reported a significant relationship between the birth month and learning capacity.

Studies conducted in Denmark, Sweden, England, and the northeast US have also reported that autistic children are born more frequently in winter and spring (42), especially in March, which may be due to poor maternal vitamin D absorption during pregnancy (43). A study by Hori et al. (44) reported a relationship between winter and early spring births and schizophrenia and schizotypal personality but found no association between intelligence and the birth season.

5.1. Conclusions

The results of this study showed a relationship between children’s ID and their parents’ old age and low education. This indicates that health organizations and institutions should undertake more concerted efforts to raise public awareness of the risk factors that are directly or indirectly associated with ID. This study did not find any relationship between the birth season and ID, but given the higher prevalence of infectious diseases, especially viral infections in cold months, it is only sensible to educate couples about how to protect the health of mother and fetus during these periods. Since this study was conducted without a control group, it was not possible to conclude about the differences between individuals with ID and those with normal intelligence. Therefore, the authors exercise caution in any generalization of the results to normal populations.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    American Psychiatric Association. "Highlights of Changes from DSM-IV to DSM-5". Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. 809 p.

  • 2.

    Schalock RL, Luckasson RA, Shogren KA, Borthwick-Duffy S, Bradley V, Buntinx WH, et al. The renaming of mental retardation: understanding the change to the term intellectual disability. Intellect Dev Disabil. 2007;45(2):116-24. [PubMed ID: 17428134]. https://doi.org/10.1352/1934-9556(2007)45[116:TROMRU]2.0.CO;2.

  • 3.

    Sogaard M, Tumer Z, Hjalgrim H, Hahnemann J, Friis B, Ledaal P, et al. Subtelomeric study of 132 patients with mental retardation reveals 9 chromosomal anomalies and contributes to the delineation of submicroscopic deletions of 1pter, 2qter, 4pter, 5qter and 9qter. BMC Med Genet. 2005;6:21. [PubMed ID: 15904506]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC1174871]. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2350-6-21.

  • 4.

    Reichenberg A, Cederlof M, McMillan A, Trzaskowski M, Kapra O, Fruchter E, et al. Discontinuity in the genetic and environmental causes of the intellectual disability spectrum. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(4):1098-103. [PubMed ID: 26711998]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4743770]. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1508093112.

  • 5.

    Belkady B, Elkhattabi L, Elkarhat Z, Zarouf L, Razoki L, Aboulfaraj J, et al. Chromosomal Abnormalities in Patients with Intellectual Disability: A 21-Year Retrospective Study. Hum Hered. 2018;83(5):274-82. [PubMed ID: 31064002]. https://doi.org/10.1159/000499710.

  • 6.

    Curry CJ, Stevenson RE, Aughton D, Byrne J, Carey JC, Cassidy S, et al. Evaluation of mental retardation: recommendations of a Consensus Conference: American College of Medical Genetics. Am J Med Genet. 1997;72(4):468-77. [PubMed ID: 9375733]. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(19971112)72:4<468::aid-ajmg18>3.0.co;2-p.

  • 7.

    Saadat M. Consanguineous marriages in Iranian folktales. Community Genet. 2007;10(1):38-40. [PubMed ID: 17167249]. https://doi.org/10.1159/000096280.

  • 8.

    Narimani M, Amani M. Mental Retardation and its related factors in Ardabil. J Ardabil Univ Med Sci. 2007;7(2):190-5.

  • 9.

    Fernell E, Bejerot S, Westerlund J, Miniscalco C, Simila H, Eyles D, et al. Autism spectrum disorder and low vitamin D at birth: a sibling control study. Mol Autism. 2015;6:3. [PubMed ID: 25874075]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4396835]. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-6-3.

  • 10.

    Eyles DW, Burne TH, McGrath JJ. Vitamin D, effects on brain development, adult brain function and the links between low levels of vitamin D and neuropsychiatric disease. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2013;34(1):47-64. [PubMed ID: 22796576]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2012.07.001.

  • 11.

    Sajjadian N, Shajari H, Mofid R, Jahadi R, Alizadeh Taheri P. The relation of increasing serum bilirubin during 24-48 hours of birth and birth season. Tehran Univ Med J. 2013;70(12).

  • 12.

    Mohammadi M, Alijanpour M, Ajdari M. Seasonal incidence of type I diabetes mellitus in the north of Iran. Caspian J Pediatr. 2018;4(2):320-3.

  • 13.

    Pinborough-Zimmerman J, Bilder D, Bakian A, Satterfield R, Carbone PS, Nangle BE, et al. Sociodemographic risk factors associated with autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. Autism Res. 2011;4(6):438-48. [PubMed ID: 21905245]. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.224.

  • 14.

    Adler A. The general system of individual psychology. In: Henry T, editor. Stein. United stait of America: The Classical Adlerian Translation Project; 2006.

  • 15.

    Botzet LJ, Rohrer JM, Arslan RC. Analysing effects of birth order on intelligence, educational attainment, big five and risk aversion in an Indonesian sample. Eur J Pers. 2020;35(2):234-48. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2285.

  • 16.

    Esposito L, Kumar SM, Villaseñor A. The importance of being earliest: birth order and educational outcomes along the socioeconomic ladder in Mexico. J Popul Econom. 2020;33(3):1069-99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-019-00764-3.

  • 17.

    Akhavan KS, Golestan M, Fallah R, Behnaz F. Evaluation of Inheritance Pattern in Mentally Retarded Children. J Shahid Sadoughi Univ Med Sci. 2011;19(3):292-301.

  • 18.

    Ayoglu FN, Cabuk F, Kiran S, Ocakci A, Sahin Z, Dursun A. The prevalence of mental retardation by gender, age of diagnosis and location in Zonguldak province, Turkey. Neurosciences (Riyadh). 2008;13(1):57-60. [PubMed ID: 21063288].

  • 19.

    Noori M, Bakhshi H, Vakilian AR. Survey the Frequency of Mental Retardation Associated Factors among Students of Rafsanjan Exceptional Schools in 2014. J Rafsanjan Univ Med Sci. 2015;13(10):1007-12.

  • 20.

    Koirala NR, Das AK, Bhagat SK. The prevalence of mental retardation by gender, age, and age of diagnosis at Nobel Medical College, Biratnagar. J Nobel Med Coll. 2012;1(2):77-81. https://doi.org/10.3126/jonmc.v1i2.7304.

  • 21.

    Axmon A, Sandberg M, Ahlstrom G. Gender differences in psychiatric diagnoses in older people with intellectual disability: a register study. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):192. [PubMed ID: 28532510]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5441013]. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1353-8.

  • 22.

    Stannard S, Berrington A, Alwan N. Associations between birth order with mental wellbeing and psychological distress in midlife: Findings from the 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70). PLoS One. 2019;14(9). e0222184. [PubMed ID: 31527876]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6748419]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0222184.

  • 23.

    Easey KE, Mars B, Pearson R, Heron J, Gunnell D. Association of birth order with adolescent mental health and suicide attempts: a population-based longitudinal study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;28(8):1079-86. [PubMed ID: 30604131]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6675759]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1266-1.

  • 24.

    Turner T, Pihur V, Chakravarti A. Quantifying and modeling birth order effects in autism. PLoS One. 2011;6(10). e26418. [PubMed ID: 22039484]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3198479]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026418.

  • 25.

    Ugur C, Tonyali A, Goker Z, Uneri OS. Birth order and reproductive stoppage in families of children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatr Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;29(4):509-14. https://doi.org/10.1080/24750573.2018.1457489.

  • 26.

    Martin LA, Horriat NL. The effects of birth order and birth interval on the phenotypic expression of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One. 2012;7(11). e51049. [PubMed ID: 23226454]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3511407]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051049.

  • 27.

    He P, Chen G, Wang Z, Guo C, Zheng X. The role of parental education in child disability in China from 1987 to 2006. PLoS One. 2017;12(10). e0186623. [PubMed ID: 29040333]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5645139]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186623.

  • 28.

    Ghafouri-Fard S, Dianatpour M, Tabei SMB, Miryounesi M. Mental retardation due to chromosomal translocation in an Iranian consanguineous family: report of three cases. Tehran Univ Med J TUMS Publ. 2017;74(11):817-22.

  • 29.

    Wang Y, McDermott S, Mann JR, Hardin JW. Preventing intellectual disability during pregnancy: what are the potentially high yield targets? J Perinat Med. 2016;44(4):421-32. [PubMed ID: 26352060]. https://doi.org/10.1515/jpm-2015-0059.

  • 30.

    Huang J, Zhu T, Qu Y, Mu D. Prenatal, Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors for Intellectual Disability: A Systemic Review and Meta-Analysis. PLoS One. 2016;11(4). e0153655. [PubMed ID: 27110944]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC4844149]. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153655.

  • 31.

    Kakaberaie K. Comparison of Biological Characteristics in Mothers of Mental Retardation and Mothers of Normal Children. Q J Health Breeze. 2014;2(2):12-9.

  • 32.

    Soleimani M, Abdi K, Moosavy Khatat M. Comparison of demographic and biological characteristics, between parents who have more than one exceptional children with parents of normal children. J Rehabil. 2011;11(5):94-104.

  • 33.

    Seltzer MM, Floyd F, Song J, Greenberg J, Hong J. Midlife and aging parents of adults with intellectual and developmental disabilities: impacts of lifelong parenting. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2011;116(6):479-99. [PubMed ID: 22126660]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3263696]. https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-116.6.479.

  • 34.

    Zhang SL. [A study on effects of parents age, birth order and mental retardation of unknown etiology]. Zhonghua Shen Jing Jing Shen Ke Za Zhi. 1992;25(5):303-5. 318. [PubMed ID: 1291254].

  • 35.

    Sandin S, Hultman CM, Kolevzon A, Gross R, MacCabe JH, Reichenberg A. Advancing maternal age is associated with increasing risk for autism: a review and meta-analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(5):477-486 e1. [PubMed ID: 22525954]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.02.018.

  • 36.

    Idring S, Magnusson C, Lundberg M, Ek M, Rai D, Svensson AC, et al. Parental age and the risk of autism spectrum disorders: findings from a Swedish population-based cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(1):107-15. [PubMed ID: 24408971]. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt262.

  • 37.

    Heuvelman H, Abel K, Wicks S, Gardner R, Johnstone E, Lee B, et al. Gestational age at birth and risk of intellectual disability without a common genetic cause. Eur J Epidemiol. 2018;33(7):667-78. [PubMed ID: 29214412]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC6061122]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-017-0340-1.

  • 38.

    Sandin S, Schendel D, Magnusson P, Hultman C, Suren P, Susser E, et al. Autism risk associated with parental age and with increasing difference in age between the parents. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21(5):693-700. [PubMed ID: 26055426]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC5414073]. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.70.

  • 39.

    Kastley M. Season of Birth Effects in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Honors Scholar Theses. 2008. Available from: http://digitalcommons.uconn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1038&context=srhonors_theses.

  • 40.

    Zerbo O, Iosif AM, Delwiche L, Walker C, Hertz-Picciotto I. Month of conception and risk of autism. Epidemiology. 2011;22(4):469-75. [PubMed ID: 21543984]. [PubMed Central ID: PMC3296777]. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31821d0b53.

  • 41.

    Liu W, He Z, Chen W, Li C, Kan H, Weschler LB, et al. Birth month is associated with learning capacity in childhood in Northeast China. Indoor Air. 2020;30(1):31-9. [PubMed ID: 31541483]. https://doi.org/10.1111/ina.12604.

  • 42.

    Maimburg RD, Bech BH, Vaeth M, Moller-Madsen B, Olsen J. Neonatal jaundice, autism, and other disorders of psychological development. Pediatrics. 2010;126(5):872-8. [PubMed ID: 20937652]. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0052.

  • 43.

    Hebert KJ, Miller LL, Joinson CJ. Association of autistic spectrum disorder with season of birth and conception in a UK cohort. Autism Res. 2010;3(4):185-90. [PubMed ID: 20803726]. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.136.

  • 44.

    Hori H, Teraishi T, Sasayama D, Matsuo J, Kawamoto Y, Kinoshita Y, et al. Relationships between season of birth, schizotypy, temperament, character and neurocognition in a non-clinical population. Psychiatry Res. 2012;195(1-2):69-75. [PubMed ID: 21824667]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.028.